Bhatia2018JEPLMC
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition
Sudeep Bhatia
Online First Publication, July
1
9, 2018. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000618
CITATION
Bhatia, S. (2018, July 19). Semantic Processes in Preferential Decision Making. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000618
Semantic Processes in Preferential Decision Making
Sudeep Bhatia
University of Pennsylvania
This article examines how semantic memory processes influence the items that are considered by
decision makers in memory-based preferential choice. Experiments 1A through 1C ask participants to list
the choice items that come to their minds while deliberating in a variety of everyday choice settings.
These experiments use semantic space models to quantify the semantic relatedness between pairs of
retrieved items and find that choice item retrieval displays robust semantic clustering effects, with
retrieved items increasing the retrieval probabilities of related items. Semantic clustering can be
disassociated from the effect of item desirability and can lead to inefficiencies such as the consideration
and evaluation of undesirable items early on in the decision. Experiments 2A through 2C use a similar
approach to study the effects of contextual cues on item retrieval and find that decision makers are biased
toward retrieving choice items that are semantically related to the choice context. This effect is usually
strongest early on in deliberation and weakens as additional items are retrieved. Overall, the results
highlight the role of semantic memory processes in guiding the generation of memory-based choice sets,
and illustrate the value of semantic space models for studying preferential decision making.
Keywords: decision making, semantic memory, semantic clustering, preregistration, open science
Preferential decision making involves the selection of a favored
item from a set of feasible choice items. Most experiments on
preferential decision processes explicitly present a choice set to
participants. Correspondingly, psychological theories of decision
making have been concerned primarily with how decision makers
choose between such an exogenously determined set of items, that
is, the decision rules they use to evaluate these items, as well as the
effects of the composition of the choice set, and other related
contextual factors, on their choices (Busemeyer & Rieskamp,
2014; Oppenheimer & Kelso, 2015).
However, many common decision scenarios do not involve a
fixed, exogenous set of choice items. Rather, decision makers must
construct such choice sets by themselves, typically through the use
of memory processes (see Alba & Hutchinson, 1987; Lynch &
Srull, 1982 for early discussions). Consider, for example, the task
of planning what to eat, buying a gift for a friend or family
member, or deciding on a vacation destination. In such settings, the
set of items that decision makers choose between is determined by
the set of items that comes to their mind. Decision makers may be
exposed to various external information sources, but in the absence
of such information, the items that do come to mind, and thus form
the choice set, are the items that are successfully retrieved from
memor
y.
The key role of memory in generating choice sets in common
choice tasks raises a number of important questions at the inter-
section of memory and decision making research. From a theoret-
ical perspective: What are the mechanisms that determine the
items that are retrieved by decision makers when exogenous
choice sets are not provided? How do these mechanisms relate to
core memory processes known to play a role in nonpreferential
choice tasks, and do these memory processes facilitate or hinder
efficient memory retrieval for decision making? Practically, can
the mechanisms at play in memory-based decision making be
tested? The set of retrieved choice items in everyday decision
making tasks is completely unconstrained—any choice item can
come to mind, and the items that do come to mind often lack a
clear category structure. So how can the relationship between the
various retrieved items, and between these items and other relevant
variables (such as choice context), be quantified?
In this article, I attempt to address these questions using existing
insights on human memory. The task of retrieving a feasible set of
choice items from memory has similarities to well-studied memory
tasks such as free recall and free association. Thus it is likely that
both the mechanisms that guide retrieval in these tasks, as well as
the effects generated by these mechanisms, carry over to the
domain of preferential decision making. For example, as with free
recall and free association, the generation of memory-based choice
sets may involve associative activation processes (Anderson, Both-
ell, Lebiere, & Matessa, 1998; Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968; Hintz-
man, 1984; Polyn, Norman, & Kahana, 2009). This would cause
memory-based choice sets to display semantic clustering, with
retrieved items increasing the retrieval probability of other seman-
tically related items (Bousfield & Sedgewick, 1944; Gruenewald
& Lockhead, 1980; Howard & Kahana, 2002; Romney, Brewer, &
Batchelder, 1993). For this reason, one would also expect retrieved
items to depend on contextual cues, such as choice context, with
items that are semantically related to these cues being more likely
to be retrieved (Hare, Jones, Thomson, Kelly, & McRae, 2009;
Funding for Sudeep Bhatia was received from the National Science
Foundation grant SES-1626825.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sudeep
Bhatia, Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, D22 Sol-
omon Labs, 3720 Walnut Street, Philadelphia, PA 19104. E-mail:
bhatiasu@sas.upenn.edu
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition
© 2018 American Psychological Association 2018, Vol. 1, No. 999, 000
0278-7393/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000618
1
mailto:bhatiasu@sas.upenn.edu
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000618
Moss, Ostrin, Tyler, & Marslen-Wilson, 1995; Nelson, McEvoy,
& Schreiber, 2004).
Moreover, it may also be possible to apply the methodological
tools developed to study memory processes in free recall and free
association tasks to the domain of preferential choice. For exam-
ple, memory researchers have previously used semantic space
models (e.g., Jones & Mewhort, 2007; Landauer & Dumais, 1997)
to evaluate the semantic relatedness between items, and between
items and contextual cues (e.g., Hills, Jones, & Todd, 2012;
Howard & Kahana, 2002). The power and generalizability of these
models implies that they could also be used to describe the se-
mantic relationships at play when decision makers are asked to
generate choice sets from memory.
Of course, it may also be possible that the types of memory
processes outlined in the preceding text play only a small role, or
no role whatsoever, in preferential choice. After all, making pref-
erential choices requires the selection of items based on their
desirability, and decision makers may be adept at retrieving only
the most desirable items from their memories, ignoring semantic
structure. It may even be possible that desirability-based retrieval
leads to the appearance of semantic clustering, implying that the
effect of desirability must be carefully controlled for when ana-
lyzing retrieval dynamics.
In either case, the study of semantic memory processes in
memory-based choice is necessary for building richer, more com-
prehensive, and more generalizable theories of preferential choice.
It can also facilitate the deeper integration of research on memory
and research on decision making, two important but separate areas
of investigation in psychology. Finally, the study of memory-based
choice can be used to determine whether or not memory processes
lead to efficient decision making, and in turn, can generate prac-
tical insights about how to improve the choices of individuals.
The goal of this article is to perform such a study. For this
purpose, it presents the results of six preregistered experiments
investigating the effect of semantic structure when decision mak-
ers are asked to deliberate about preferential choice items in the
absence of exogenously provided choice sets. Experiments 1A
through 1C test for semantic clustering in such memory-based
choice sets. These experiments also examine the relationship be-
tween retrieval dynamics and choice item desirability. Experi-
ments 2A through 2C expand on this work by considering the
impact of choice context on the generation of memory-based
choice sets. Both sets of experiments use semantic space models to
quantify the semantic relatedness between pairs of items and
between items and the choice context, thereby allowing for a
rigorous analysis of the effects of semantic structure in an other-
wise unconstrained choice item domain.
Retrieving Items From Memory
The study of how individuals retrieve items from memory has a
long history in psychological research. Memory retrieval has been
examined using a wide variety of different tasks, including free
recall from lists, free recall of natural categories, and free associ-
ation. In the first task individuals are presented with a list of items
(usually common words) and are asked to recall all the items on
the list that they are able to. In the second task, individuals are not
given a list, but are rather ask to list all exemplars of a given
category (e.g., “animals”) that they can think of. In the third task,
individuals are merely asked to list all the items that come to their
mind as they are presented with a specific prompt (e.g., “dog”; see
Kahana, 2012 for a comprehensive review).
These three tasks shed light on the organization of memory, and
suggest a major role for semantic memory processes in item retrieval.
For example, free recall and free association tasks are vulnerable to
semantic clustering effects according to which items that are similar to
each other are retrieved alongside each other (Bousfield & Sedge-
wick, 1944; Gruenewald & Lockhead, 1980; Howard & Kahana,
2002; Howard, Jing, Addis, & Kahana, 2007; Romney et al., 1993;
Wixted & Rohrer, 1994). Additionally, varying contextual cues, such
as prompts in free association tasks, can bias retrieval in favor of items
that are semantically similar to the prompt (Hare et al., 2009; Moss et
al., 1995; Nelson, McEvoy, & Dennis, 2000; Roediger, Watson,
McDermott, & Gallo, 2001). These effects are often modeled using
theories of associative memory (Anderson et al., 1998; Atkinson &
Shiffrin, 1968; Hintzman, 1984; Polyn et al., 2009). According to
these theories memory is probed with cues that activate closely
associated items. As items are retrieved, they themselves cue subse-
quent retrieval, leading to semantic clustering and other complex
dynamics.
The types of tasks used in the aforementioned work closely
mimic common decision scenarios in which decision makers have
to choose between items in the absence of exogenous choice sets
(Alba & Hutchinson, 1987; Lynch & Srull, 1982). If similar
memory mechanisms are at play in both settings, then many of the
semantic effects observed in free recall and free association should
carry over to the preferential choice domain. To test for these
effects, I consider memory-based decisions in which individuals
are asked to list the items that come to their mind while making
choices in various everyday scenarios, such as deciding what to
eat, what to buy as a gift, and where to go for a vacation. I expect
to observe both semantic clustering, with items that are semanti-
cally related to the previously retrieved item being more likely to
be retrieved, and context dependence, with items that are seman-
tically related to the choice context being most likely to be re-
trieved.
Semantic Space Models
Memory-based choice tasks are relatively unconstrained, and
decision makers can list almost any item (or combination of items)
that they think of. The unconstrained nature of these tasks imposes
the challenge of quantifying semantic relatedness: How can one
measure semantic clustering or the effect of choice context if the
items generated in the task are not predetermined by the experi-
menter?
Fortunately, researchers have already proposed a solution to this
problem: semantic space models (Griffiths, Steyvers, & Tenen-
baum, 2007; Jones & Mewhort, 2007; Kwantes, 2005; Landauer &
Dumais, 1997; Lund & Burgess, 1996; Mikolov, Sutskever, Chen,
Corrado, & Dean, 2013). Such models use word co-occurrence
statistics in large natural language data sets to derive high-
dimensional semantic vectors for a large set of words. Semanti-
cally related words have vectors that are close to each other, so that
semantic relatedness between nearly any pair of words can be
quantified by vector distance between their corresponding vectors.
Semantic space models have been shown to predict a wide range
of psychological phenomena involving similarity judgment, cate-
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
2 BHATIA
gorization, text comprehension, and semantic priming (Griffiths et
al., 2007; Jones & Mewhort, 2007; Landauer & Dumais, 1997;
also see Bullinaria & Levy, 2007 or Jones, Willits, Dennis, &
Jones, 2015 for a review). Critically, they have also been shown to
be successful at specifying semantic relatedness, and, for this
reason, they can predict the set of listed items (as well as the order
in which these items are listed) in completely unconstrained free
recall and free association tasks (e.g., Hills et al., 2012; Hills,
Todd, & Jones, 2015). Finally, recent work has found that seman-
tic space models are also able to describe high-level cognitive
phenomena related to decision making such as factual judgment,
probability judgment, forecasting, and social judgment (Bhatia,
2017a, 2017b), suggesting that such models may also be applicable
to memory-based preferential choice.
In this article, I apply pretrained semantic vectors obtained
though the Word2Vec methods of Mikolov et al. (2013). These
vectors were generated by applying a recurrent neural network on
a corpus of Google News articles with over 100 billion words.
They have a vocabulary of 300 million words and phrases, with
each word or phrase being defined on 300 dimensions. I specify
semantic relatedness using cosine similarity. Thus for any pair of
items x and y listed in the experiments, I measure the semantic
relatedness of x and y by calculating S(x, y) � x · y/(||x|| · ||y||),
where x and y are the Word2Vec vectors corresponding to items x
and y. S(x, y) ranges from �1 for vectors that are in identical
directions, to �1 for vectors that are in opposite directions (S[x, y] �
0 corresponds to orthogonal vectors). Note that, occasionally, I had
to compute semantic relatedness when decision makers mention
items composed of multiple words. In these settings, I merely
average the vectors of the component words to obtain a single
composite vector representation for the item (which is passed
through the preceding cosine similarity function to compute se-
mantic relatedness). I also use this approach to measure the se-
mantic relatedness between any item and a given choice context
(such as dinner or breakfast in a task asking decision maker to
consider what to eat). Thus for an item x and a context cue c, I
calculate the semantic relatedness of x and c using S(x, c).
Desirability
The setting I am examining does have one important feature not
usually at play in common memory tasks: the effect of item
desirability. Decision makers need to retrieve items in order to find
items to choose. Thus efficient decision making, which involves
the selection of the most desirable item as quickly as possible,
requires the retrieval of the most desirable items first. Some prior
work on memory processes suggests that item desirability may
play a role in item retrieval. For example, emotionality has been
shown to influence retrieval probability in free-recall tasks, with
strongly valenced or arousing items being more likely to be re-
trieved earlier in the task (e.g., Doerksen & Shimamura, 2001;
Rubin & Friendly, 1986; but see Talmi & Moscovitch, 2004).
However, it is not clear whether desirability has a similar effect in
the decision making context, and whether this effect is so strong so
as to wash out the semantic components of retrieval.
In fact, the potential effect of desirability on retrieval can lead to
incorrect conclusions about underlying semantic processes, if peo-
ple have similar preferences for similar items. Consider, for ex-
ample, a decision maker who likes pasta dishes more than salad
dishes. For this decision maker, the best possible meal choice is
pasta Bolognese, followed by pasta Alfredo, and then Mediterra-
nean salad and Caesar salad. If this decision maker retrieves food
items purely in order of their desirability, one would still expect to
observe some degree of semantic clustering; that is, similar items
(which are similarity desirable) will be retrieved successively.
However, this type of clustering would emerge even if semantic
processes were not directly at play in the task.
Controlling for the effects of desirability on item retrieval, the
present experiments involve evaluations of desirability for all
items that were considered by the decision maker. I do expect
that—all else equal—items rated as being more desirable will be
retrieved first, but my primary analysis involves the additional
effect of semantic relatedness. Thus, I test for whether semantic
clustering and related effects emerge even when the effect of
desirability is controlled for.
Note that the emergence of a semantic clustering effect, in
addition to a desirability effect, would imply a type of inefficiency
in the decision process. If decision makers cluster items by seman-
tic proximity, they may occasionally retrieve undesirable items
that are semantically related to previously retrieved items before
retrieving highly desirable but semantically unrelated items. This
can increase the time necessary to make the optimal choice (or
conversely increase the likelihood of making a suboptimal choice).
Memory and Decision Making
There has already been some research examining free recall in
a marketing context. This work is concerned primarily with the
recall of brands, and the effects of various marketing-related
variables (e.g., order of entry into the market or exposure to
advertisements) on the ability of consumers to successfully re-
trieve brands (Hutchinson, Raman, & Mantrala, 1994; Nedungadi,
1990; Shapiro, MacInnis, & Heckler, 1997; see also Roberts &
Lattin, 1997 for a review). Some of this research suggests that
brand recall dynamics display semantic clustering, with brands
within the same product category being more likely to be retrieved
together (Hutchinson, 1983; Lattin & Roberts, 1992). The setting
that I am concerned with is more general than brand recall (par-
ticipants can list any items that come to their mind as they
deliberate about an everyday decision), and additionally examines
the effect of contextual cues on item listing. For this reason, this
analysis of participant behavior also requires the use of semantic
space models to specify semantic relatedness, a technique that has
not been used in the study of brand recall. However, despite these
theoretical and methodological differences, the existing research
does suggest that the semantic processes at play in free recall
settings may also extend to preferential decision making and that
one could expect to observe effects such as semantic clustering in
these experiments.
Another set of research related to these tests involves theoretical
models of decision making based on memory processes. There are
a number of such models that allow for contextual cues and related
probes to influence the retrieval of decision-relevant information
(Dougherty, Gettys, & Ogden, 1999; Glöckner, Hilbig, & Jekel,
2014; Johnson, Häubl, & Keinan, 2007; Marewski & Schooler,
2011). However, the model that is most relevant to these hypoth-
eses is the associative accumulation model (Bhatia, 2013), which
allows choice items and contextual cues to bias attribute activation
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
3SEMANTIC PROCESSES IN DECISION MAKING
and in turn bias the activation of other choice items, in a manner
that is very similar to standard associative memory models (such
as, e.g., Polyn et al., 2009). For this reason, the Associative
Accumulation Model can also be seen to predict the emergence of
semantic clustering and context dependence in decisions from
memory.
Experiments 1A, 1B, and 1C: Semantic Clustering
Experiments 1A, 1B, and 1C test for semantic clustering effects
in settings in which decision makers have to retrieve choice items
from memory. These effects involve the influence of retrieved
items on successive retrieval and predict that semantically related
items should be retrieved one after the other. All three of these
experiments involve identical procedures and vary only in terms of
the decision domain, and thus I present their methods and results
simultaneously. Note that all three experiments have been prereg-
istered and approved by the University of Pennsylvania Institu-
tional Review Board (IRB).1
Method
Participants. Participants (N � 50; mean age � 29; 42%
female) in Experiment 1A, participants in Experiment 1B (N � 50;
mean age � 30; 30% female), and participants in Experiment 1C
(N � 50; mean age � 30; 44% female), recruited from Prolific
Academic, performed the experiment online. All participants were
residents of the United States. They were compensated at a rate of
approximately $US7.50/hr.
Procedures. In all three experiments participants were shown
a description of a decision setting and were asked to list 20 items
that came to their mind as they considered making their decisions.
The instructions used were as follows:
Experiment 1A (foods): Imagine that you could eat anything that you
wanted for a meal tomorrow. In the boxes below please list 20 food
items that come to your mind as you consider what to eat. Please make
sure to list all foods that you think of, regardless of whether you would
eventually want to eat them. Please list these foods in the order that
they come to your mind (i.e., the first food that comes to your mind
listed first, the second listed second, etc.)
Experiment 1B (vacations): Imagine that you could go on any vaca-
tion that you wanted. In the boxes below please list 20 vacation
destinations that come to your mind as you consider where to go.
Please make sure to list all destinations that you think of, regardless
of whether you would eventually want to go the destination. Please list
these vacation destinations in the order that they come to your mind
(i.e., the first destination that comes to your mind listed first, the
second listed second, etc.)
Experiment 1C (gifts): Imagine that you have to purchase a gift for a
close friend or family member. In the boxes below please list 20 items
that come to your mind as you consider what gift to purchase. Please
make sure to list all items that you think of, regardless of whether you
would eventually want to purchase them. Please list these items in the
order that they come to your mind (i.e., the first item that comes to
your mind listed first, the second listed second, etc.)
Participants listed all the items on a single screen, in successive
free entry boxes. After these items were listed participants were
taken to a second screen on which they rated each of their 20 listed
items in terms of desirability, on a scale from �3 to � 3 (�3
corresponding to extremely undesirable, 0 to neither desirable nor
undesirable, and � 3 to extremely desirable). Thus participants in
Experiment 1A rated their 20 items based on how much they
would like to eat each of these items, participants in Experiment
1B rated their items based on how much they would like to go
there for a vacation, and participants in Experiment 1C rated their
items based on how much they would like to give the item as a gift.
Results
Conditional response probabilities. To test whether the
items listed by participants were clustered semantically, I first
analyzed the data using the approach suggested by Howard and
Kahana (2002). This approach tests the relationship between the
retrieval order and the similarity of the items retrieved, as mea-
sured by latent semantic analysis (LSA; Landauer & Dumais,
1997). More specifically it defines a measure: the LSA conditional
response probability (LSA-CRP), which specifies the probability
of retrieving one item given the previously retrieved item as a
function of the LSA-based cosine similarities of the two items.
This involves first dividing the pairwise similarities between all
the items into a small number of equally sized bins, based on the
magnitude of similarity (with the first bin corresponding to the
smallest pairwise cosine similarity values, and the last bin corre-
sponding to the largest pairwise cosine similarity values). Once
these bins have been generated, LSA-CRP computes the relative
frequencies of the similarities of successively retrieved items fall-
ing into each of these bins (ensuring that the measure only con-
siders items that are available to be retrieved at each point in time).
If there are no systematic semantic clustering effects, one would
expect the conditional response probabilities for each bin to be
roughly identical; that is, items that are semantically related to the
previously retrieved item (i.e., items whose similarities with the
retrieved item are in bins with large cosine similarity values)
should have a similar probability of being retrieved as items that
are semantically unrelated to the previously retrieved item (i.e.,
items whose similarities with the retrieved item are in bins with
small cosine similarity values). In contrast, in the presence of
semantic clustering, the conditional response probabilities should
be highest for items whose similarities fall into the highest bins.
Howard and Kahana did find that CRPs correlate positively with
bin magnitudes, rigorously demonstrating semantic clustering ef-
fects in free recall.
The analysis I performed in Experiments 1A through 1C was
identical to that performed by Howard and Kahana (2002), except
for one minor difference. Howard and Kahana applied their ap-
proach to free recall from lists, and used the pairwise similarities
between every word in their word pool to determine the pairwise
cosine similarity bins. In contrast, these experiments allowed de-
cision makers to list any items. Thus, I was unable to precompute
pairwise similarity bins. Instead, for each participant, I took the 20
items listed by that participant and computed the pairwise simi-
larities between these items, generating 190 distinct pairwise sim-
ilarities. I then used these 190 pairwise similarities to generate 10
separate bins, with the first bin containing the smallest pairwise
similarity values and the last bin containing the largest pairwise
1 Preregistration materials are available at: https://osf.io/zudp7/
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
4 BHATIA
https://osf.io/zudp7/
similarity values (see Hills et al., 2012 and 2015 for a similar
approach to studying semantic clustering in unconstrained tasks).
Overall, I found a strong positive relationship between CRP and
the Word2Vec-based similarity as measured by the approach out-
lined in the preceding text. This is shown in Figures 1A through 1C
which plot the average CRPs across participants in each experi-
ment, for each of the 10 bins. As can be seen in these figures, the
average CRPs for the highest bins (corresponding to the largest
pairwise similarity values) were substantially higher than the av-
erage CRPs for the remaining bins, for all three experiments. This
indicates that after retrieving an item, decision makers were espe-
cially likely to retrieve other semantically related items (items
whose pairwise similarities with the retrieved item are in the
highest bin). Overall, I observed a correlation of 0.85 between bin
number (1, 2,. . . 10) and the average CRP for Experiment 1A
(foods), a correlation of 0.69 for Experiment 1B (vacations), and a
correlation of 0.73 for Experiment 1C (gifts).
The preceding analysis aggregates CRPs for participants for
each bin, however, a more rigorous test should also consider
individual heterogeneity in the CRP-bin correlations. For this
purpose, I ran a series of regressions (one for each experiment), in
which the dependent variables were the calculated CRP values and
the independent variables were the bin numbers. These regressions
considered each CRP for each participant’s bin to be a separate
observation, and also permitted random effects on the participant
level. These regressions found a very strong positive effect for all
three experiments (� � 0.008, z � 7.52, p � 0.001, 95% CI �
[0.006, 0.010] for Experiment 1A; � � 0.008, z � 7.78, p � 0.001,
95% CI � [0.006, 0.010] for Experiment 1B; � � 0.014, z �
11.33, p � 0.001, 95% CI � [0.012, 0.017] for Experiment 1C),
again providing evidence for semantic clustering in the generation
of memory-based choice sets.
Path analysis. Yet another approach to measuring semantic
clustering has been proposed by Romney et al. (1993). Romney et
al. (1993) used multidimensional scaling to derive pairwise seman-
tic distance measures for all items in a free recall from lists task.
They then computed the total distance traveled in a participant’s
retrieved item path. This distance is the sum of the distances
between the first and second retrieved item, the second and the
third retrieved item, the third and fourth retrieved item, and so on.
To quantify the extent of semantic clustering, they compared the
participant’s actual distance with the distance that would be ex-
pected if the participant retrieved items in a random order. The
latter can be calculated through Monte Carlo methods.
To ensure the robustness of the CRP-based analysis performed
previously, I applied Romney et al.’s (1993) techniques to the
present dataset. Again this required two minor modifications. First,
instead of using multidimensional scaling-based distances, I used
a measure D(x,y) � 1 – S(x,y), with vectors x and y derived from
the preexisting Word2Vec representations, and S(x,y) correspond-
ing to the cosine similarity between these vectors. D is bounded
below by 0 and is thus a suitable measure of semantic distance (as
assessed on the Word2Vec space). Second, as the set of items
listed by participants was unconstrained (unlike Romney et al.’s
set, which was determined by a list of items presented to partici-
pants prior to the recall task), I computed random path distance
only on the set of items listed by each participant. This implies that
these tests compared the participant’s actual path over that partic-
ipant’s retrieved items, with a random path over the same set of
retrieved items. In order to calculate these random paths, I con-
ducted 100,000 simulations, with each simulation generating a
random ordering over the set of items retrieved by the participant.
Our path analysis again showed strong effects of semantic
clustering. This is illustrated in Figures 2A through 2C which plot
each participant’s actual distance against the mean distance of a
random path on the participant’s set of listed items. As is shown in
these figures, the actual distances for participants were almost
always shorter than the distances of the corresponding random
paths. Specifically, 88% of participants in Experiment 1A, 96% of
participants in Experiment 1B, and 94% of participants in Exper-
iment 1C had shorter actual distances than mean random distances
(p � .001 for all three experiments according to a binomial test).
Figure 1. A (top), B (middle), and C (bottom): The average conditional
response probability (CRP) for each of the 10 pairwise similarity bins for
Experiments 1A through 1C. Errors bars display �/–1 standard error.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
5SEMANTIC PROCESSES IN DECISION MAKING
Using a paired t test to compare the actual distances with the mean
random distances, across participants, also showed that on aggre-
gate these distances were significantly shorter, t(49) � 8.84, p �
0.001 for Experiment 1A; t(49) � 10.09, p � 0.001 for Experi-
ment 1B; t(49) � 9.54, p � 0.001 for Experiment 1C.2
Desirability. A crucial variable missing from the preceding
tests is item desirability. As discussed in the introductory sections
of this article, efficient decision making involves the retrieval of
the most desirable items first, and it is possible that a desirability-
based retrieval strategy could indirectly generate the appearance of
semantic clustering (without any semantic processes directly at
play).
A simple analysis examining the relationship between item
desirability and retrieval order showed that desirability does play a
role in retrieval. This is illustrated in Figures 3A through 3C,
which plots the desirabilities of items retrieved in each retrieval
position, averaged across participants. As can be seen here, the
items retrieved first had a high average desirability and the items
retrieved last had a low average desirability (though these items
were still rated to be somewhat desirable, rather than undesirable).
Overall there was a correlation of �0.74 between item order (1,
2,. . . 20) and average item desirability in Experiment 1A (foods),
a correlation of �0.93 in Experiment 1B (vacations), and a cor-
relation of �0.76 in Experiment 1C (gifts). I also tested the
desirability effect by performing a regression analysis over all the
data, in which the dependent variable was retrieval order (1, 2, . . .
20), and the independent variable was the participant’s rated item
desirability, and each observation corresponded to a single re-
trieved item for a single participant. This analysis controlled for
participant-level heterogeneity with random effects, and found
a significant effect of desirability on retrieval order for all three
experiments (� � �0.60, z � �4.48, p � 0.001, 95% CI
[�0.86, �0.34] for Experiment 1A; � � �1.30, z � �8.53,
p � 0.001, 95% CI [�1.60, �1.00] for Experiment 1B;
� � �0.51, z � �4.28, p � 0.001, 95% CI [�0.75, �0.28] for
Experiment 1C).
One way to control for the effect of desirability on retrieval, in
order to examine semantic clustering effects independent of desir-
2 Note that the preregistration analysis plan had specified the use of z
tests for each participant, however a paired t test is better suited to
evaluating differences across participants in the experiment. For this rea-
son, I report only the paired t test here.
Figure 2. A (top left), B (top right), and C (bottom): Scatter plots of distances of participants’ sequence of
retrieved items versus average distances if sequence order was randomized, for Experiments 1A through 1C.
Each point corresponds to a single participant in each of the three experiments.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
6 BHATIA
ability, is to perform a variant of the path analysis presented in the
previous section. Here, instead of comparing the participant’s
actual semantic distance with a random path distance, one can
compare the participants’ actual distance with the distance of a
retrieval path that lists items in order of their desirability (with the
most desirable item listed first, the second most desirable item
listed second, and so on). If the participant’s path distance is
shorter than the desirability path distance, one can conclude that
semantic clustering effects emerge even when controlling for the
type of artificial semantic clustering that can be generated by a
purely desirability-based retrieval strategy. In fact, one can also
compare the desirability path distance with the random path dis-
tance to quantify the extent of this type of indirect desirability-
based semantic clustering.
Note that the desirability rating task offered participants a scale
with seven points (�3 to �3 in increments of 1), and for this
reason, many items were given the same rating by the participant.
Thus there are multiple possible desirability-based paths, with each
path permitting a different ordering (but nonetheless restricting the
ordering so that more desirable items are always listed before less
desirable items). Thus to calculate the mean desirability-based path
distance across all feasible desirability-based paths, I again used
Monte Carlo methods with 100,000 simulated desirability-based
orderings. For each simulation, I generated a strict ordering for
listed items based on their desirability ratings, with the positioning
of identically rated items being randomized. The overall desirabil-
ity path distance for the simulation was calculated as the sum of
the distances of adjacent items in the ordering.
A comparison between the participants’ actual distances and the
mean desirability path distances is provided in Figures 4A through
4C. As can be seen in these figures, actual distances were almost
always shorter than mean desirability path distances. Specifically,
82% of participants in Experiment 1A, 88% of participants in
Experiment 1B, and 88% of participants in Experiment 1C had
shorter actual distances than mean desirability distances (p � .001
for all three experiments according to a binomial test). Again a
paired t test confirmed that these difference were significant,
t(49) � 6.59, p � 0.001 for Experiment 1A; t(49) � 7.56, p �
0.001 for Experiment 1B; t(49) � 7.93, p � 0.001 for Experiment
1C.3 A similar paired t test comparing average desirability paths
with average random paths also found that desirability paths were
shorter than random paths, supporting my earlier claim that a
purely desirability-based retrieval strategy can indirectly lead to
the appearance of semantic clustering, t(49) � 3.23, p � 0.01 for
Experiment 1A; t(49) � 1.64, p � .11 for Experiment 1B; t(49) �
6.21, p � 0.001 for Experiment 1C. Note that this difference does
not reach statistical significance for Experiment 1B.
Now, the finding that the actual paths of decision makers were
different from the desirability paths suggests that undesirable items
were being retrieved earlier than if decision makers were able to
retrieve items only in order of desirability. To rigorously examine
this, I compared the relationship between desirability and retrieval
order (as in Figures 3A through 3C) with the relationship that one
would expect if the items were retrieved strictly in order of their
desirability. Specifically, for each participant, I reordered the re-
trieved set of items based on their desirabilities to generate an ideal
ordering, in which the most desirable item is retrieved first, the
Figure 3. A (top), B (middle), and C (bottom): The average desirability
ratings for items listed in different positions (1 for listed first to 20 for
listed last), for Experiments 1A, 1B, and 1C. A rating of �3 corresponds
to extremely desirable and a rating of 0 corresponds to neither desirable nor
undesirable. Errors bars display �/–1 standard error.
3 The preregistration analysis plan had specified the use of a linear
regression, instead of this path analysis. In this regression, each observation
would capture the semantic similarity (dependent variable), ordering dis-
tance (independent variable), and desirability difference (control variable)
between each pair of listed items for each participant. Although such a
regression does reveal a significant negative relationship between ordering
distance and semantic similarity, controlling for desirability, I have real-
ized that this regression is not statistically sound, as observations cannot be
considered to be independent (i.e. the semantic similarity of items A and B
and B and C influences the semantic similarity of items A and C). The path
analysis provides a better way of disentangling desirability from semantic
clustering, which is why I have listed the path analysis results here.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
7SEMANTIC PROCESSES IN DECISION MAKING
second most desirable item is retrieved second, and so on. I then
subtracted the desirability of each item in each of the positions in
the participant’s retrieved list from the desirability of the item in
the corresponding position in the ideal list. This gave us a list with
differences in desirabilities in each of the 20 retrieval positions.
If decision makers did retrieve undesirable items earlier and
desirable items later (than ideal), then the differences in this list
would be decreasing in order (e.g., a positive difference would
be observed for the first position and a negative difference
would be observed for the last position). I tested for this
decreasing relationship in regressions in which the dependent
variable was the desirability difference and the independent vari-
able was the corresponding order position. These regressions consid-
ered each of the 20 retrieval positions for each of the participants to be
a separate observation, and controlled for participant-heterogeneity
with random effects. They revealed a significant negative effect of
position for all three experiments (� � �2.76, z � �26.92, p �
0.001, 95% CI [�2.96, �2.56] for Experiment 1A; � � �3.44,
z � �30.19, p � 0.001, 95% CI [�3.67, �3.22] for Experiment 1B;
� � �2.94, z � �38.04, p � 0.001, 95% CI [�3.09, �2.79] for
Experiment 1C). This confirms that in the present experiments, un-
desirable items were being retrieved significantly earlier than ideal
(i.e., significantly earlier than if decision makers were able to retrieve
items strictly in order of desirability).4
Discussion
The goal of Experiments 1A through 1C was to test for semantic
clustering effects in the retrieval of choice sets from memory. It
used two existing techniques to quantify semantic clustering: the
first, based on the conditional response probability approach out-
lined by Howard and Kahana (2002), involved testing whether the
retrieval of one item increased the likelihood of similar items being
retrieved in the successive time period. The second, based on the
path analysis approach outlined by Romney et al. (1993) involved
calculating the total distance traveled in semantic space by a
participant’s retrieval sequence, and comparing this to the distance
expected if retrieval order was randomized.
Both of these techniques indicated strong semantic clustering
effects in Experiments 1A through 1C, however neither of these
4 This analysis was not mentioned in the preregistered analysis plan, but
is necessary for testing whether undesirable items were retrieved earlier
than would be predicted by desirability-based retrieval.
Figure 4. A (top left), B (top right), and C (bottom): Scatter plots of distances of participants’ sequence of
retrieved items versus average distances if items were listed in order of desirability, for Experiments 1A, 1B, and
1C. Each point corresponds to a single participant in each of the three experiments.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
8 BHATIA
techniques controlled for the effect of desirability on retrieval.
Desirable items were more likely to be retrieved first, suggesting
that the semantic clustering observed using the CRP and the path
analysis techniques could be due to decision makers merely having
similar preferences for similar items, and not due to any direct
semantic effects in retrieval. To control for desirability, I thus ran
a variant of the path analysis that calculated the average semantic
distances of retrieval sequences that listed items purely in order of
desirability. I found that such desirability-based distances were
still much larger than the distances of the participants’ actual
retrieval sequences, suggesting the existence of direct semantic
clustering effects in addition to desirability-based retrieval biases.
The presence of semantic clustering suggests that undesirable
items that are semantically related to previously retrieved items
can occasionally intrude and be retrieved before more desirable
(but semantically unrelated) items. This hints at a potential inef-
ficiency in the decision process—With perfect memory retrieval,
decision makers would be quicker and less prone to error if they
did not cluster retrieval semantically, and instead first considered
the most desirable items before examining other less desirable
items.
Experiments 2A, 2B, and 2C: Choice Context
In Experiments 1A through 1C, there was a robust semantic
clustering effect, showing that items that are semantically related
to previously retrieved items are themselves more likely to be
retrieved in successive time periods. This illustrates the key role of
semantic relatedness in determining the items that are considered
by decision makers in decisions from memory. In Experiments 2A
through 2C, I examine a second semantic effect, this time involv-
ing the influence of choice context on the retrieval of items. These
experiments consider choice domains that are identical to those
used in Experiments 1A through 1C but vary the contextual cues
given to participants in these domains. If semantic processes do
guide retrieval, one should expect items that are semantically
related to the choice context to be more likely to be retrieved.
Experiments 2A through 2C involve identical procedures to
each other, and vary only in terms of the decision domain, and thus
I present their methods and results simultaneously. Again, all three
experiments have been preregistered and approved by the Univer-
sity of Pennsylvania IRB.5
Method
Participants. Participants (N � 100; mean age � 31; 44%
female) in Experiment 2A, participants in Experiment 2B (N �
100; mean age � 30; 47% female), and participants in Experiment
2C (N � 100; mean age � 30; 41% female), recruited from
Prolific Academic, performed the experiment online. All partici-
pants were residents of the United States. They were compensated
at a rate of approximately $US7.50/hr.
Procedures. In all three experiments participants were shown
a description of a decision setting and were asked to list 20 items
that came to their mind as they considered making their decisions.
The specific choice context used in the decision setting was,
however, randomly assigned to participants, with each participant
receiving one of two contextual cues. The instructions (with the
randomized choice context underlined) were as follows:
Experiment 2A (foods): Imagine that you could eat anything that you
wanted for [dinner/breakfast] tomorrow. In the boxes below please list
20 food items that come to your mind as you consider what to eat.
Please make sure to list all foods that you think of, regardless of
whether you would eventually want to eat them. Please list these foods
in the order that they come to your mind (i.e., the first food that comes
to your mind listed first, the second listed second).
Experiment 2B (vacations): Imagine that you could go on a [wine
tasting/camping] trip to any destination that you wanted. In the boxes
below please list 20 vacation destinations that come to your mind as
you consider where to go. Please make sure to list all destinations that
you think of, regardless of whether you would eventually want to go
the destination. Please list these vacation destinations in the order that
they come to your mind (i.e., the first destination that comes to your
mind listed first, the second listed second, etc.).
Experiment 2C (gifts): Imagine that you have to purchase a gift for
your [partner for Valentine’s day/friend for her baby shower]. In the
boxes below please list 20 items that come to your mind as you
consider what gift to purchase. Please make sure to list all items that
you think of, regardless of whether you would eventually want to
purchase them. Please list these items in the order that they come to
your mind (the first item that comes to your mind listed first, the
second listed second).
Participants listed all the items on a single screen. After these
items were listed participants were taken to a second screen on
which they rated each of their 20 items for desirability in their
corresponding choice context, on a scale from �3 to �3 (�3
corresponding to extremely undesirable, 0 to neither desirable nor
undesirable, and �3 to extremely desirable). Thus participants in
Experiment 2A who were given the dinner context rated their 20
items based on how much they would like to eat each of the items
for dinner, and participants in this experiment who were given the
breakfast context rated their items on how much they would like to
eat each of the items for breakfast. Likewise, participants in
Experiment 2B rated their items based on how much they would
like to go there for a wine tasting or a camping trip, and partici-
pants in Experiment 2C rated their items based on how much they
would like to give the item as a Valentine’s day or baby shower
gift.
Results
Context dependence. Our primary tests for Experiments 2A
through 2C involved the effect of the choice context on the set of
retrieved items. For this purpose, I used the Word2Vec represen-
tations to obtain vectors for each pair of contextual cues.6 I then
calculated the relative cosine similarities between these vectors
and each of the items listed by the participants to measure the
relative semantic relatedness between listed items and the two
choice contexts in each of the three experiments. More formally,
for each item x in Experiment 2A, I calculated S(x, dinner) – S(x,
breakfast), for each item x in Experiment 2B, I calculated S(x, wine
tasting) – S(x, camping), and for each item x in Experiment 2C, I
5 Preregistration materials are available at: https://osf.io/zudp7/.
6 Note that the Word2Vec representations had individual vectors for
Valentines_Day, baby_Shower, and wine_tasting, and I used these individ-
ual vectors instead of averaging the vectors for component words in these
phrases.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
9SEMANTIC PROCESSES IN DECISION MAKING
https://osf.io/zudp7/
calculated S(x, Valentine’s day) – S(x, baby shower). I then com-
pared these relative cosine similarity values across the two groups
of participants in each experiment. If choice context does influence
the items retrieved, one would expect S(x, dinner) – S(x, breakfast) to
be larger for participants in Experiment 2A in the dinner context
relative to the breakfast context. Likewise S(x, wine tasting) – S(x,
camping) should be larger for participants in Experiment 2B in the
wine tasting context relative to the camping context, and S(x, Valen-
tine’s day) – S(x, baby shower) should be larger for participants in
Experiment 2C in the Valentine’s day context relative to the baby
shower context.
The relationship between item retrieval and choice context can
be seen in Figures 5A through 5C, which plot the average cosine
similarity differences across participants, for the three experi-
ments, as a function of condition. The solid lines always lie above
the dashed lines, indicating that retrieved items were more similar
to their choice context compared with the choice context in the
alternate condition. More rigorously, I tested for these differences
using three regressions (one for each experiment) in which the
main dependent variable was the cosine similarity difference (S[x,
dinner] – S[x, breakfast] for Experiment 2A, S[x, wine tasting] –
S[x, camping] for Experiment 2B, and S[x, Valentine’s day] – S[x,
baby shower] for Experiment 2C) and the main independent vari-
able was the experimental conditions to which the participants
were assigned (1 if condition was dinner in Experiment 2A, wine
tasting in Experiment 2B, and Valentine’s Day in Experiment 2C;
0 otherwise). I also controlled for participant-heterogeneity with
random effects. This regression illustrated strong positive effects
of condition on cosine similarity, showing that choice context does
in fact influence item retrieval (� � 0.04, z � 9.16, p � 0.001,
95% CI [0.03, 0.05] for Experiment 2A; � � 0.08, z � 8.33, p �
0.001, 95% CI [0.06, 0.10] for Experiment 2B; � � 0.08, z �
15.72, p � 0.001, 95% CI [0.07, 0.09] for Experiment 2C).
We also ran variants of the preceding regressions with three
additional independent variables: the first variable was the re-
trieval position of the item (1 to 20, based on whether the item was
listed first or last). The second variable was an interaction between
retrieval position and the condition variable. This variable tested
whether the effect of choice context diminished or increased as
additional items were retrieved. The final variable was the choice
item’s rated desirability. I found that the effect of the condition
variable on cosine similarity persisted with these additional cova-
riates (p � 0.001 for all). Moreover, there was a negative inter-
action effect between the condition and the item ordering in
Experiments 2B (vacations) and 2C (gifts), indicating that the
effect of the choice context diminished as additional items were
retrieved (� � �0.002, z � �2.94, p � 0.01, 95% CI
[�0.003, �0.001] for Experiment 2B; � � �0.006, z � �9.90, p �
0.001, 95% CI [�0.007, �0.005] for Experiment 2C). This effect can
also be observed in Figures 5B and 5C which show that the differ-
ences in average cosine similarities between the solid and dashed lines
decrease for items retrieved later on during deliberation. There was no
such interaction effect for Experiment 2A (foods; p � .42).
Semantic clustering and desirability. We also analyzed the
data from Experiments 2A through 2C to test if the semantic
clustering effects observed previously were replicated. For this
purpose, I analyzed each of the conditions in each of the experi-
ments separately. I first calculated the relationship between con-
ditional response probabilities and sematic relatedness with re-
trieved items (in a manner that was identical to that reported for
Experiments 1A through 1C). For this, I ran six regressions (one
for each condition in each experiment), in which the dependent
variables were the calculated CRP values and the independent
variables were the bin numbers (we also included random effects
on the participant-level). As in Experiments 1A through 1C, these
regressions found a very strong positive effect of semantic relat-
edness on CRP (p � 0.001 for all six tests).
We replicated this result using the path analysis approach out-
lined in the preceding text. For this, I again compared the semantic
Figure 5. A (top), B (middle), and C (bottom): The similarity differences
for items listed in different positions (1 for listed first to 20 for listed last),
as a function of condition in Experiments 2A, 2B, and 2C. Here the solid
lines correspond to choice contexts of dinner, wine tasting, and Valentine’s
day, and dashed lines correspond to contexts of breakfast, camping, and
baby shower, in Experiment 2A, 2B, and 2C respectively. Errors bars
display �/–1 standard error.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
10 BHATIA
distance over the path of items listed by participants with the
distance expected if item retrieval order was random and if the
item retrieval order was determined entirely in order of desirability
(with the most desirable item listed first). Again, I found that the
actual distances were lower than random distances and desirability
distances, indicating the presence of semantic clustering. These
differences were significant, as evaluated by paired t tests across
participants in each condition in each experiment (p � 0.001 for all
six tests).
A final analysis involved examining the effect of desirability on
retrieval order. For this purpose, I ran a regression analysis in
which the dependent variable was retrieval order (1 to 20, based on
whether the item was listed first or last), and the independent
variable was the participant’s rated item desirability. I also in-
cluded a control for the cosine similarity of the item with the
contextual cue. This analysis counted each retrieved item by each
participant as a separate observation and controlled for participant-
heterogeneity with random effects. Replicating the findings of
Experiments 1A through 1C, I found positive significant effects for
desirability (p � 0.001 for all six tests), indicating that desirable
items are more likely to be retrieved earlier, even when controlling
for their similarity with the choice context.7
Discussion
Experiments 2A through 2C tested for the effect of contextual
cues on item retrieval. They specified semantic relatedness using
cosine similarity between Word2Vec word vectors. As expected
they found that decision makers were more likely to retrieve items
that are semantically related to the choice context. Additionally,
the effect of choice context was stronger for items retrieved early
on during deliberation for Experiments 2B (vacations) and 2C
(gifts). As deliberation continued, retrieved items in these exper-
iments drifted further away from the contextual cue. Additionally,
as in Experiments 1A through 1C, Experiments 2A through 2C
also noted a strong degree of semantic clustering, with retrieved
items increasing the likelihood of semantically related items being
retrieved in the subsequent time period. I also observed the effect
of desirability on item retrieval: Highly desirable items were more
likely to be retrieved early on in the decision. Overall Experiments
2A through 2C find evidence for another important semantic
determinant of item retrieval: choice context. These experiments
also illustrate the value of semantic space models for formalizing
semantic relatedness between contextual cues and item listings.
General Discussion
Semantic processes play a key role in guiding behavior in a wide
range of cognitive tasks. I have shown that these processes also
influence the choice items that come to mind when decision
makers have to generate choice sets from memory. Particularly, in
Experiments 1A through 1C, I found that retrieved items increase
the retrieval probability of other items that they are semantically
related to. This generates a semantic clustering effect according to
which semantically related items are retrieved together. This clus-
tering effect exists in addition to desirability-based retrieval biases.
In Experiments 2A through 2C, I found that contextual cues, such
as choice context, increase the retrieval probabilities of items that
are semantically related to these cues. This effect is usually stron-
ger early on in deliberation.
Memory-Based Choice Sets
Most experimental research in psychology presents explicit
choice sets to participants, and is consequently focused on under-
standing the decision rules that are used to evaluate items in these
choice sets (see, e.g., Busemeyer & Rieskamp, 2014; Oppenheimer
& Kelso, 2015). However, many everyday decisions do not in-
volve externally provided choice sets; rather decision makers must
retrieve feasible choice items from memory (Alba & Hutchinson,
1987; Lynch & Srull, 1982). It is these decisions from memory that
are sensitive to the influence of semantic processes.
Our results suggest that to build more comprehensive theories
preferential decision making, psychologists should attempt to
model the semantic memory mechanisms responsible for choice
item retrieval in the absence of exogenous choice sets. There are
already theories of preferential choice that involve a significant
memory component, and permit the influence of probes and cues
to guide memory retrieval (e.g., Dougherty et al., 1999; Glöckner
et al., 2014; Johnson et al., 2007). Out of these, the associative
accumulation model (Bhatia, 2013) has perhaps the most explicit
semantic component for simple preferential choice, in that it al-
lows for salient choice items and other contextual variables in the
choice task to bias the activation of associated attributes, and thus
increase the activation of other (semantically related) choice items.
It is possible that such a model could be combined with existing
theories of semantic memory to more rigorously describe the
retrieval patterns documented in this article.
One benefit of this endeavor would be a more detailed analysis
of the effect of noise in memory retrieval and memory-based
choice. In the current article, I did not consider the possibility that
some of the findings could be influenced by noise in memory.
Although there is no reason to expect this type of noise to artifi-
cially generate semantic clustering or context dependence, noise
could interact in complex ways with underlying semantic structure.
A formal model of memory retrieval that allows for the effect of
noise would be able to better control for this interaction.
Semantic processes are not the only memory mechanisms at
play in memory-based decisions. The decision maker’s familiarity
with individual items (which can be measured by the frequency of
the item’s occurrence in the decision maker’s environment) is
likely to guide retrieval as well. This type of familiarity-based
memory effect has already been shown to play a role in a number
of decision making domains, and existing models of memory-
based judgment and decision making explicitly allow for the
influence of such factors on item activation and retrieval (Giger-
enzer & Goldstein, 1996; Marewski & Schooler, 2011).
Another important variable influencing the retrieval of items in
memory-based choice tasks is the desirability of the items. In these
experiments, I found that items rated as being highly desirable
were also the ones that were most likely to be listed earlier during
deliberation. The causes of this desirability bias are currently
unclear. For example, desirability could influence the baseline
activation of items making desirable items easier to retrieve. This
would imply that more desirable items are also more likely to be
7 The semantic clustering and desirability-based analysis outlined in this
subsection was not mentioned in the preregistration analysis plan, however
it does provide a convenient replication of the results of Experiments 1A,
1B, and 1C.
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
11SEMANTIC PROCESSES IN DECISION MAKING
retrieved in memory tasks without an explicit preferential compo-
nent (congruent with the finding that word emotionality increases
word recall—see Rubin & Friendly, 1986). Alternatively, desir-
ability could act as a contextual cue; a cue that selectively in-
creases the activation of desirable choice items only when decision
makers have to make preferential decisions (see Talmi & Mosco-
vitch, 2004 for a related point). This would imply that the desir-
ability biases observed in these experiments are unique to prefer-
ential decision making contexts. Understanding the influence of
desirability on retrieval in memory-based choice, as well as the
interaction between desirability and various semantic and non-
semantic memory processes, is an important topic for future
work.
Efficient Memory
The tests performed in this article controlled for the influence of
desirability on retrieval, and demonstrated the existence of a dis-
tinguishable, direct, semantic clustering effect. One consequence
of this effect is that decision makers often retrieved undesirable
items that were semantically related to previously retrieved items
before retrieving more desirable but semantically unrelated items.
This type of semantic interference can be seen as being detrimental
to the decision: If decision makers needed to maximize accuracy
and minimize decision time, retrieval would be entirely in order of
desirability.
Although these experiments suggest the presence of a type of
inefficiency in memory-based decisions, semantic clustering can
alternatively be seen as reflecting the effortless use of an existing
memory system. For example, a purely desirability-based retrieval
strategy would likely put additional cognitive demands on the
decision maker, as decision makers would have to suppress the
associative activation processes responsible for semantic cluster-
ing, while simultaneously searching for and activating only the
most desirable items stored in their memories. In fact, to the extent
that similar items are similarly desirable, semantic clustering can
even approximate desirability-based retrieval, implying that the
effects observed in this article may reflect adaptive memory search
in decision making. Interestingly, executive control has been
shown to lead to increased semantic clustering in memory tasks
(Hills, Mata, Wilke, & Samanez-Larkin, 2013; Hills, Todd, &
Goldstone, 2010), suggesting that individuals with higher working
memory span may be especially likely to display the types of
patterns documented in this article.
Another way in which semantic processes facilitate good deci-
sions involves the use of contextual cues. As I have shown in these
experiments, decision makers selectively retrieve items that are
semantically related to the choice context. Thus when deliberating
over what to eat for breakfast, they are able to easily and quickly
retrieve breakfast-related food items. In contrast, when deliberat-
ing over what to eat for dinner, they are able to easily and quickly
retrieve dinner-related food items. Preferences for choice items
depend on context and automatically responding to choice context
likely leads quicker and more accurate decisions.
Decision Rules
A complete understanding of the relationship between semantic
memory and efficient or inefficient decision making, however,
depends on the choice rules that are used by decision makers to
evaluate retrieved items. If item retrieval is sequential then deci-
sion makers could utilize alternative-wise decision rules such as
the satisficing heuristic. Such rules consider the overall desirability
of an item and accept or reject the item before considering other
items (e.g., Simon, 1956). Alternatively, if retrieval is in parallel,
or if decision makers evaluate items only after multiple items have
been retrieved, choice may involve attribute-wise decision strate-
gies such as the lexicographic heuristic. Such rules evaluate the
attributes of choice alternatives one after the other and accept or
reject items based on their values on these attributes (e.g., Giger-
enzer & Goldstein, 1996; Tversky, 1972). Other decision rules,
involving a mix of attribute and alternative-wise processing are
also possible (see, e.g., Bhatia, 2017c; Glöckner et al., 2014; Roe,
Busemeyer, & Townsend, 2001), as are decision rules that select
“typical” items such as those that are overall most similar to the
entire set of retrieved items. It is possible that the observed
retrieval strategies are optimal with one set of decision rules but
not with another (also see Payne, Bettman, & Johnson, 1993).
Of course the study of the decision rules used by decision
makers in memory-based choice tells us about more than just
whether memory is efficient. Understanding these rules allows
researchers to fully characterize the memory-based decision pro-
cess and thus predict not only the items that decision makers
consider while deliberating, but also the items that they eventually
choose and how long they take to make their choices. Although
one would expect the findings documented here to generalize to
settings in which choices are required (as items retrieved by
decision makers fundamentally constrain their choice sets) a direct
study of decision rules used in memory-based choice can provide
a more detailed understanding of the effect of semantic clustering
and context dependence on actual decision outcomes. It is possible
that these effects are diminished with some decision rules, and
amplified with others.
Although a full characterization of the memory-based decision
process is still lacking, there has been some work that has con-
trasted memory-based decision making against stimulus-based de-
cision making in areas such as marketing (Lynch, Marmorstein, &
Weigold, 1988; Lynch & Srull, 1982; Nedungadi, 1990; Rotten-
streich, Sood, & Brenner, 2006). Much of this work involves
providing information regarding the available choice items and
their attributes to decision makers prior to the decision task, and
then asking decision makers to recall this information as they
decide. Behavior in such memory-based tasks is compared with
behavior in equivalent stimulus-based tasks in which information
is provided to decision makers as they decide (and need not be
retrieved from memory). This work has documented systematic
differences in choice strategies and choice outcomes between
memory-based and stimulus-based decisions, implying that exist-
ing psychological theories of decision making may not be directly
applicable to the types of memory-based choices studied in the
current article.
Semantic Space Models
One important contribution of this article is that it shows how
decisions involving everyday choice items can be rigorously stud-
ied with the use of semantic space models (Griffiths et al., 2007;
Jones & Mewhort, 2007; Kwantes, 2005; Landauer & Dumais,
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
12 BHATIA
1997; Lund & Burgess, 1996; Mikolov et al., 2013). These deci-
sions typically involve a large, unconstrained, choice domain: In
these experiments, participants were free to list any choice items
that came to their mind. Psychological research often has difficul-
ties quantifying relevant variables (such as the semantic related-
ness between pairs of items) in such unconstrained settings, mak-
ing naturalistic memory-based decisions very hard to study. This is
perhaps one reason why most existing decision making research
utilizes artificial experimenter provided choice sets. However,
semantic space models make it is possible to obtain semantic
representations for a very large set of words and concepts. These
representations provide measures of semantic relatedness for
nearly any pair of choice items, and between choice items and a
wide range of choice contexts.
Our application of semantic space models to measure semantic
relatedness was influenced by similar existing applications to free
recall and free association (e.g., Hills et al., 2012; Howard &
Kahana, 2002). Besides applications to memory, such models are
also useful for predicting behavior in a variety of other psycho-
logical tasks, including similarity judgment, categorization, text
comprehension, and semantic priming (see Bullinaria & Levy, 2007
or Jones et al., 2015 for a review), as well as various high-level
cognitive tasks studied by scholars of judgment and decision making
(Bhatia, 2017a, 2017b).
Of course, semantic space models do have limitations. As they
rely on large natural language corpora for model training, most
such models are unable to accommodate individual differences in
semantic representations. For example, some of the participants in
Experiment 1A may have considered rice to be more semantically
related to stir fried vegetables, whereas others may have consid-
ered it to be more semantically related to refried beans. As these
tests involved a single set of pretrained vector representations, they
predicted the same pairwise similarity measures for items across
participants, and thus were unable to accommodate any differences
in recall across participants that may have stemmed from differ-
ences in item representation. Another issue involves items com-
posed of multiple words. In these tests, I merely averaged the word
vectors for the component words in multiword items, however this
type of averaging may not always be the best way to obtain
composite representations.
Despite these limitations, semantic space models provide a
unique and promising approach to the study of preferential
choice, an area of research that is concerned primarily with how
individuals aggregate and evaluate information about real-
world items. The psychological processes involved in such
tasks cannot be easily understood by presenting decision mak-
ers with explicit choice sets composed of abstracted artificial
experimenter-generated stimuli. Rather the study of real-world
choice requires the use of both naturalistic stimuli and natural-
istic methods for eliciting preferences. By providing a way to
approximate the representations that individuals have for com-
plex everyday choice items, semantic space models allow for
the extension of existing psychological theories (such as theo-
ries of free recall) to a wide range of real-world phenomena. I
look forward to further applications of semantic space models
to the study of decision making in psychology and related
fields.
References
Alba, J. W., & Hutchinson, J. W. (1987). Dimensions of consumer exper-
tise. The Journal of Consumer Research, 13, 411– 454. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1086/209080
Anderson, J. R., Bothell, D., Lebiere, C., & Matessa, M. (1998). An
integrated theory of list memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 38,
341–380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2553
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed
system and its control processes. Psychology of Learning and Motiva-
tion, 2, 89 –195. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60422-3
Bhatia, S. (2013). Associations and the accumulation of preference. Psy-
chological Review, 120, 522–543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032457
Bhatia, S. (2017a). The semantic representation of prejudice and stereo-
types. Cognition, 164, 46 – 60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition
.2017.03.016
Bhatia, S. (2017b). Associative judgment and vector space semantics.
Psychological Review, 124, 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rev0000047
Bhatia, S. (2017c). Choice rules and accumulator networks. Decision, 4,
146 –170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dec0000038
Bousfield, W. A., & Sedgewick, C. H. W. (1944). An analysis of sequences
of restricted associative responses. The Journal of General Psychology,
30, 149 –165. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1944.10544467
Brenner, L., Rottenstreich, Y., Sood, S., & Bilgin, B. (2007). On the
psychology of loss aversion: Possession, valence, and reversals of the
endowment effect. The Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 369 –376.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/518545
Bullinaria, J. A., & Levy, J. P. (2007). Extracting semantic representations
from word co-occurrence statistics: A computational study. Behavior
Research Methods, 39, 510 –526. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193020
Busemeyer, J. R., & Rieskamp, J. (2014). Psychological research and
theories on preferential choice. In S. Hess & A. Daly (Eds.), Handbook
of choice modelling (pp. 49 –72). Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Pub-
lishing.
Doerksen, S., & Shimamura, A. P. (2001). Source memory enhancement
for emotional words. Emotion, 1, 5–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-
3542.1.1.5
Dougherty, M. R., Gettys, C. F., & Ogden, E. E. (1999). MINERVA-DM:
A memory processes model for judgments of likelihood. Psychological
Review, 106, 180 –209. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.180
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal
way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650 –
669. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.650
Glöckner, A., Hilbig, B. E., & Jekel, M. (2014). What is adaptive about
adaptive decision making? A parallel constraint satisfaction account.
Cognition, 133, 641– 666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08
.017
Griffiths, T. L., Steyvers, M., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2007). Topics in
semantic representation. Psychological Review, 114, 211–244. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211
Gruenewald, P. J., & Lockhead, G. R. (1980). The free recall of category
examples. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and
Memory, 6, 225–240. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.6.3.225
Hare, M., Jones, M., Thomson, C., Kelly, S., & McRae, K. (2009).
Activating event knowledge. Cognition, 111, 151–167. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.009
Hills, T. T., Jones, M. N., & Todd, P. M. (2012). Optimal foraging in
semantic memory. Psychological Review, 119, 431– 440. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0027373
Hills, T. T., Mata, R., Wilke, A., & Samanez-Larkin, G. R. (2013).
Mechanisms of age-related decline in memory search across the adult
life span. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2396 –2404. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0032272
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., & Goldstone, R. L. (2010). The central executive
as a search process: Priming exploration and exploitation across do-
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
13SEMANTIC PROCESSES IN DECISION MAKING
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209080
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209080
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2553
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421%2808%2960422-3
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032457
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.03.016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/rev0000047
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dec0000038
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00221309.1944.10544467
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/518545
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03193020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.1.1.5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.1.180
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.650
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.6.3.225
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.01.009
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027373
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0027373
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032272
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0032272
mains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139, 590 – 609.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020666
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., & Jones, M. N. (2015). Foraging in semantic
fields: How we search through memory. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7,
513–534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/tops.12151
Hintzman, D. L. (1984). MINERVA 2: A simulation model of human
memory. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 16,
96 –101. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03202365
Howard, M. W., Jing, B., Addis, K. M., & Kahana, M. J. (2007). Semantic
structure and episodic memory. In T. K. Landauer, D. S. McNamara, S.
Dennis, & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Handbook of latent semantic analysis (pp.
121–142). http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203936399.ch7
Howard, M. W., & Kahana, M. J. (2002). When does semantic similarity
help episodic retrieval? Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 85–98.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2798
Hutchinson, J. (1983). Expertise and the structure of free recall. Ann Abor,
MI: ACR North American Advances.
Hutchinson, J. W., Raman, K., & Mantrala, M. K. (1994). Finding choice
alternatives in memory: Probability models of brand name recall. Jour-
nal of Marketing Research, 31, 441– 461. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/
3151875
Johnson, E. J., Häubl, G., & Keinan, A. (2007). Aspects of endowment: A
query theory of value construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 461– 474. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/0278-7393.33.3.461
Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. (2007). Representing word meaning and
order information in a composite holographic lexicon. Psychological
Review, 114, 1–37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.1
Jones, M. N., Willits, J., Dennis, S., & Jones, M. (2015). Models of
semantic memory. In J. Townsend, Z. Whang, & A. Eidels (Eds.),
Oxford handbook of mathematical and computational psychology (pp.
232–254). Oxford, UK: Oxford university press.
Kahana, M. J. (2012). Foundations of human memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford
university press.
Kwantes, P. J. (2005). Using context to build semantics. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 12, 703–710. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196761
Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem:
The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and rep-
resentation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211
Lattin, J. M., & Roberts, J. H. (1992). Testing for probabilistic indepen-
dence in consideration of ready-to-eat cereals. Palo Alto, CA: Graduate
School of Business, Stanford University.
Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic
spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Instru-
ments, & Computers, 28, 203–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF0
3204766
Lynch, J. G., Jr., Marmorstein, H., & Weigold, M. F. (1988). Choices from
sets including remembered brands: Use of recalled attributes and prior
overall evaluations. The Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 169 –184.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209155
Lynch, J. G., Jr., & Srull, T. K. (1982). Memory and attentional factors in
consumer choice: Concepts and research methods. The Journal of Con-
sumer Research, 9, 18 –37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208893
Marewski, J. N., & Schooler, L. J. (2011). Cognitive niches: An ecological
model of strategy selection. Psychological Review, 118, 393– 437. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024143
Mikolov, T., Sutskever, I., Chen, K., Corrado, G. S., & Dean, J. (2013).
Distributed representations of words and phrases and their composition-
ality. In C. J. C. Burges, L. Bottou, M. Welling, Z. Ghahramani, & K. Q.
Weinberger (Eds), Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems
(pp. 3111–3119). Red Hook, NY: Curran associates.
Moss, H. E., Ostrin, R. K., Tyler, L. K., & Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1995).
Accessing different types of lexical semantic information: Evidence
from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Mem-
ory, and Cognition, 21, 863– 883. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-
7393.21.4.863
Nedungadi, P. (1990). Recall and consumer consideration sets: Influencing
choice without altering brand evaluations. The Journal of Consumer
Research, 17, 263–276. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208556
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Dennis, S. (2000). What is free associ-
ation and what does it measure? Memory & Cognition, 28, 887– 899.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209337
Nelson, D. L., McEvoy, C. L., & Schreiber, T. A. (2004). The University
of South Florida free association, rhyme, and word fragment norms.
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 402– 407.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195588
Oppenheimer, D. M., & Kelso, E. (2015). Information processing as a
paradigm for decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 277–
294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015148
Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive
decision maker. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Polyn, S. M., Norman, K. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2009). A context
maintenance and retrieval model of organizational processes in free
recall. Psychological Review, 116, 129 –156. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1037/a0014420
Roberts, J. H., & Lattin, J. M. (1997). Consideration: Review of research
and prospects for future insights. Journal of Marketing Research, 34,
406 – 410. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3151902
Roe, R. M., Busemeyer, J. R., & Townsend, J. T. (2001). Multialternative
decision field theory: A dynamic connectionist model of decision mak-
ing. Psychological Review, 108, 370 –392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0033-295X.108.2.370
Roediger, H. L., III, Watson, J. M., McDermott, K. B., & Gallo, D. A.
(2001). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression anal-
ysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 385– 407. http://dx.doi.org/10
.3758/BF03196177
Romney, A. K., Brewer, D. D., & Batchelder, W. H. (1993). Predicting
clustering from semantic structure. Psychological Science, 4, 28 –34.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00552.x
Rottenstreich, Y., Sood, S., & Brenner, L. (2006). Feeling and thinking in
memory-based versus stimulus-based choices. Journal of Consumer
Research, 33, 461– 469.
Rubin, D. C., & Friendly, M. (1986). Predicting which words get recalled:
Measures of free recall, availability, goodness, emotionality, and pro-
nunciability for 925 nouns. Memory & Cognition, 14, 79 –94. http://dx
.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209231
Shapiro, S., MacInnis, D. J., & Heckler, S. E. (1997). The effects of
incidental ad exposure on the formation of consideration sets. The
Journal of Consumer Research, 24, 94 –104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/
209496
Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment.
Psychological Review, 63, 129 –138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
h0042769
Talmi, D., & Moscovitch, M. (2004). Can semantic relatedness explain the
enhancement of memory for emotional words? Memory & Cognition,
32, 742–751. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195864
Tversky, A. (1972). Elimination by aspects: A theory of choice. Psycho-
logical Review, 79, 281–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0032955
Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (1994). Analyzing the dynamics of free recall:
An integrative review of the empirical literature. Psychonomic Bulletin
& Review, 1, 89 –106. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200763
Received December 27, 2017
Revision received March 20, 2018
Accepted March 24, 2018 �
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
14 BHATIA
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0020666
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/tops.12151
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03202365
http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9780203936399.ch7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2798
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3151875
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3151875
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.461
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.461
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.1.1
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196761
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.211
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03204766
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03204766
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209155
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208893
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024143
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0024143
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.863
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.863
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/208556
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209337
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195588
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015148
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014420
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014420
http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/3151902
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.370
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.370
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196177
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196177
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00552.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209231
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209231
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209496
http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/209496
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0042769
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0042769
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03195864
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0032955
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03200763
Why Work with Us
Top Quality and Well-Researched Papers
We always make sure that writers follow all your instructions precisely. You can choose your academic level: high school, college/university or professional, and we will assign a writer who has a respective degree.
Professional and Experienced Academic Writers
We have a team of professional writers with experience in academic and business writing. Many are native speakers and able to perform any task for which you need help.
Free Unlimited Revisions
If you think we missed something, send your order for a free revision. You have 10 days to submit the order for review after you have received the final document. You can do this yourself after logging into your personal account or by contacting our support.
Prompt Delivery and 100% Money-Back-Guarantee
All papers are always delivered on time. In case we need more time to master your paper, we may contact you regarding the deadline extension. In case you cannot provide us with more time, a 100% refund is guaranteed.
Original & Confidential
We use several writing tools checks to ensure that all documents you receive are free from plagiarism. Our editors carefully review all quotations in the text. We also promise maximum confidentiality in all of our services.
24/7 Customer Support
Our support agents are available 24 hours a day 7 days a week and committed to providing you with the best customer experience. Get in touch whenever you need any assistance.
Try it now!
How it works?
Follow these simple steps to get your paper done
Place your order
Fill in the order form and provide all details of your assignment.
Proceed with the payment
Choose the payment system that suits you most.
Receive the final file
Once your paper is ready, we will email it to you.
Our Services
No need to work on your paper at night. Sleep tight, we will cover your back. We offer all kinds of writing services.
Essays
No matter what kind of academic paper you need and how urgent you need it, you are welcome to choose your academic level and the type of your paper at an affordable price. We take care of all your paper needs and give a 24/7 customer care support system.
Admissions
Admission Essays & Business Writing Help
An admission essay is an essay or other written statement by a candidate, often a potential student enrolling in a college, university, or graduate school. You can be rest assurred that through our service we will write the best admission essay for you.
Reviews
Editing Support
Our academic writers and editors make the necessary changes to your paper so that it is polished. We also format your document by correctly quoting the sources and creating reference lists in the formats APA, Harvard, MLA, Chicago / Turabian.
Reviews
Revision Support
If you think your paper could be improved, you can request a review. In this case, your paper will be checked by the writer or assigned to an editor. You can use this option as many times as you see fit. This is free because we want you to be completely satisfied with the service offered.