EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY TASK SWITCHING

ONE PARAGRAPH ANSWERING BOTH QUESTIONS

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

1. Define and discuss the idea of a task set with an example (refer

to research that has been done by others)

2. Define and discuss the idea of task set reconfiguration, and

explain how this idea explains the task switching cost. (also

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

define switch cost)

[FIRST READING (INTRO MONSELL) IS BEST, BUT IF YOU NEED MORE INFO THE SECOND ONE CAN HELP. PLEASE IGNORE THE HIGHLIGHTED AREAS]

  • Task switching
  • Stephen Monsell

    School of Psychology University of Exeter, Exeter, EX4 4QG, UK

    Everyday life requires frequent shifts between cognitive
    tasks. Research reviewed in this article probes the con-
    trol processes that reconfigure mental resources for a
    change of task by requiring subjects to switch fre-
    quently among a small set of simple tasks. Subjects’
    responses are substantially slower and, usually, more
    error-prone immediately after a task switch. This
    ‘switch cost’ is reduced, but not eliminated, by an
    opportunity for preparation. It seems to result from
    both transient and long-term carry-over of ‘task-set’
    activation and inhibition as well as time consumed by
    task-set reconfiguration processes. Neuroimaging
    studies of task switching have revealed extra activation
    in numerous brain regions when subjects prepare to
    change tasks and when they perform a changed task,
    but we cannot yet separate ‘controlling’ from ‘con-
    trolled’ regions.

    A professor sits at a computer, attempting to write a paper.
    The phone rings, he answers. It’s an administrator,
    demanding a completed ‘module review form’. The pro-
    fessor sighs, thinks for a moment, scans the desk for the
    form, locates it, picks it up and walks down the hall to the
    administrator’s office, exchanging greetings with a col-
    league on the way. Each cognitive task in this quotidian
    sequence – sentence-composing, phone-answering, con-
    versation, episodic retrieval, visual search, reaching and
    grasping, navigation, social exchange – requires an
    appropriate configuration of mental resources, a pro-
    cedural ‘schema’ [1] or ‘task-set’ [2]. The task performed
    at each point is triggered partly by external stimuli (the
    phone’s ring and the located form). But each stimulus
    affords alternative tasks: the form could also be thrown in
    the bin or made into a paper plane. We exercise intentional
    ‘executive’ control to select and implement the task-set,
    or the combination of task-sets, that are appropriate to
    our dominant goals [3], resisting temptations to satisfy
    other goals.

    Goals and tasks can be described at multiple grains or
    levels of abstraction [4]: the same action can be described
    as both ‘putting a piece of toast in one’s mouth’ and
    ‘maintaining an adequate supply of nutrients’. I focus here
    on the relatively microscopic level, at which a ‘task’
    consists of producing an appropriate action (e.g. conveying
    to mouth) in response to a stimulus (e.g. toast in a
    particular context). One question is: how are appropriate
    task-sets selected and implemented? Another is: to what
    extent can we enable a changed task-set in advance of the
    relevant stimulus – as suggested by the term ‘set’?

    Introspection indicates that we can, for example, set
    ourselves appropriately to name a pictured object aloud
    without knowing what object we are about to see. When an
    object then appears, it is identified, its name is retrieved
    and speech emerges without a further ‘act of intention’: the
    sequence of processes unfolds as a ‘prepared reflex’ [5,6].

    Many task-sets, which were initially acquired through
    instruction or trial and error, are stored in our memories.
    The more we practice a task, or the more recently we have
    practised it, the easier it becomes to re-enable that task-
    set. At the same time, in the absence of any particular
    intention, stimuli we happen to encounter evoke ten-
    dencies to perform tasks that are habitually associated
    with them: we unintentionally read the text on cereal
    packages or retrieve the names of people we pass in the
    street. More inconveniently, stimuli evoke the tendency to
    perform tasks habitually associated with them despite a
    contrary intention. The standard laboratory example of
    this is the Stroop effect [7]: we have difficulty suppressing
    the reading of a colour name when required to name the
    conflicting colour in which it is printed (e.g. ‘RED’ printed
    in blue). Brain damage can exacerbate the problem, as in
    ‘utilization behaviour’, which is a tendency of some
    patients with frontal-lobe damage to perform the actions
    afforded by everyday instruments, such as matches,
    scissors and handles, even when these actions are
    contextually inappropriate [8].

    Hence the cognitive task we perform at each moment,
    and the efficacy with which we perform it, results from
    a complex interplay of deliberate intentions that are
    governed by goals (‘endogenous’ control) and the avail-
    ability, frequency and recency of the alternative tasks
    afforded by the stimulus and its context (‘exogenous’
    influences). Effective cognition requires a delicate, ‘just-
    enough’ calibration of endogenous control [9] that is
    sufficient to protect an ongoing task from disruption
    (e.g. not looking up at every movement in the visual
    field), but does not compromise the flexibility that allows
    the rapid execution of other tasks when appropriate
    (e.g. when the moving object is a sabre-toothed tiger).

    To investigate processes that reconfigure task-set, we
    need to induce experimental subjects to switch between
    tasks and examine the behavioural and brain correlates of
    changing task. Task-switching experiments are not new
    (Box 1), but the past decade has seen a surge of interest,
    stimulated by the development of some novel techniques
    for inducing task switches and getting subjects to prepare
    for them (Box 2), and some surprising phenomena revealed
    thereby, as well as by the broader growth of interest in
    control of cognition (e.g. [10]).Corresponding author: Stephen Monsell (s.monsell@ex.ac.uk).

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003134

    http://tics.trends.com

    1364-6613/03/$ – see front matter q 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7

    http://www.trends.com

    Mobile User

    Mobile User

    Mobile User

    Mobile User

    Mobile User

    Mobile User
    1. Pt 2

    Task switching: basic phenomena
    In a task-switching experiment, subjects are first pre-
    trained on two or more simple tasks afforded by a set of
    stimuli (Figs 1 and 2 provide examples). Each task
    requires attention to, and classification of, a different
    element or attribute of the stimulus, or retrieval from
    memory or computation of a different property of the
    stimulus. Then, a stimulus is presented on each of a series
    of trials and the subject performs one of the tasks. There
    are several methods for telling the subject which task to
    perform (Box 2), but in all cases the task sometimes
    changes from one trial to the next, and sometimes does not.
    Thus, we can examine performance or brain activation on
    and following trials when the task changes for evidence of
    extra processing demands that are associated with the
    need to reconfigure task-set. We can also examine the
    effects of localized brain damage, transient magnetic
    stimulation (TMS) or pharmacological interventions on
    behavioural indices of switching efficiency. Four phenom-
    ena of primary interest (of which the first three are
    illustrated in Figs 1 and 2) are described below.

    Switch cost (task-repetition benefit)
    Generally, responses take longer to initiate on a ‘switch trial’
    than on a ‘non-switch’ or task-repetition trial, often by a
    substantial amount (e.g. 200 ms relative to a baseline of
    500 ms).Also,theerrorrateisoftenhigherafterataskswitch.

    Preparation effect
    If advance knowledge is given of the upcoming task and
    time allowed to prepare for it, the average switch cost is
    usually reduced.

    Residual cost
    Preparation generally does not eliminate the switch cost.
    In the examples shown, the reduction in switch cost seems
    to have reached a substantial asymptote, the ‘residual

    cost’, after ,600 ms of preparation. Substantial residual
    costs have been reported even when 5 s or more is allowed
    for preparation (e.g. [11,12]).

    Mixing cost
    Although performance recovers rapidly after a switch
    (Fig. 1), responses remain slower than when just one task
    must be performed throughout the block: there is a long-
    term as well as a transient cost of task switching.

    These phenomena have been demonstrated with a wide
    range of different tasks and they are modulated by
    numerous other variables. What explains them?

    Sources of the switch cost
    Time taken by control operations
    To change tasks, some process or processes of ‘task-set
    reconfiguration’ (TSR) – a sort of mental ‘gear changing’ –
    must happen before appropriate task-specific processes
    can proceed. TSR can include shifting attention between
    stimulus attributes or elements, or between conceptual
    criteria, retrieving goal states (what to do) and condition–
    action rules (how to do it) into procedural working memory
    (or deleting them), enabling a different response set and
    adjusting response criteria. TSR may well involve inhi-
    bition of elements of the prior task-set as well as activation
    of the required task-set.

    An account of the switch cost that appeals intuitively is
    that it reflects the time consumed by TSR. The preparation
    effect then suggests that, if sufficient time is allowed, TSR
    can, to some extent, be accomplished under endogenous
    control, before the stimulus onset. The residual cost is
    more perplexing. Rogers and Monsell [13] suggest that

    Box 1. Early research on task-set and task switching

    The intentional and contextual control of ‘set’ (‘Einstellung’) was
    discussed in 19th and early 20th century German experimental
    psychology. In 1895, von Kries used as examples the way the clef sign
    changes the action performed to play a note on the musical stave, and
    the way the current state of a game changes how one sets oneself to
    respond to an opponent’s behaviour [58]. Exner and the Wurzburg
    school described the ‘prepared reflex’, and, in 1910, Ach described
    experiments on overlearned responses competing with the acqui-
    sition of a novel stimulus–response mapping, see [6]. Until recently,
    in the English-language literature, ideas about control of task-set have
    been stimulated mainly by the observation of impairments of control,
    both in everyday action and as a result of neurological damage, see
    [2], despite some experimentation on normal executive function in
    cognitive laboratories [5].

    The invention of the task-switching paradigm is credited to Jersild
    [59] who had students time themselves working through a list of
    items, either repeating one task or alternating between two. Some
    task pairs (adding 3 to vs. subtracting 3 from numbers) resulted in
    dramatic alternation costs; others (adding 3 to a number vs. writing
    the antonym of an adjective) did not. Jersild’s paradigm was revived,
    and his results replicated using discrete reaction-time measurements,
    by Biederman and Spector [60]. Despite this work and some
    pioneering task-cueing studies (e.g. [61–63]) it was only in the mid
    1990s that the present surge of research on task switching developed.

    Box 2. Task switching paradigms

    There are several methods of telling a subject which task to do on each
    trial. Jersild’s method (Box 1), which is still sometimes used (e.g. [39]),
    compares the duration of blocks of trial in which the subject alternates
    tasks as rapidly as possible with blocks in which they repeat just one
    task. This contrast of alternated and repeated tasks can also be used
    with discrete reaction-time measurement (e.g. [14]). However, this
    comparison confounds switch costs and mixing costs. Also, the
    alternation blocks impose a greater working memory load – to keep
    track of the task sequence and maintain two tasks in a state of
    readiness – and might promote greater effort and arousal. These
    problems are avoided by the alternating-runs paradigm [13], in which
    the task alternates every N trials, where N is constant and predictable
    (e.g. Fig. 1, predictable condition, and Fig. 2), so that one can compare
    task-switch and task-repetition trials within a block. An alternative is to
    give the subjects short sequences of trials [20,27] with a prespecified
    task sequence (e.g. colour–shape–colour). Either way, one can
    manipulate the available preparation time by varying the stimulus–
    response interval, but this also varies the time available for any
    passive dissipation of the previous task-set.

    In the task-cueing paradigm [63,64], the task is unpredictable, and
    a task cue appears either with or before the stimulus (e.g. Fig. 1,
    random condition). It is now possible to manipulate independently
    the cue–stimulus interval (allowing active preparation) and the
    response–cue interval (allowing passive dissipation). Alternatively,
    in the intermittent-instruction paradigm, the series of trials is
    interrupted occasionally by an instruction that indicates which task
    to perform on the trials following the instruction [65]. Even when the
    instruction specifies continuing with the same task, there is a ‘restart’
    cost after the instruction [29], but this is larger when the task changes;
    the difference yields a measure of switch cost.

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003 135

    http://tics.trends.com

    http://www.trends.com

    Mobile User

    part of TSR cannot be done until exogenously triggered
    by stimulus attributes that are associated with the
    task; Rubinstein et al. [14] characterize this part as
    retrieval of stimulus–response rules into working
    memory. An alternative account, from De Jong [15],
    makes no distinction between endogenous and exogen-
    ously-triggered TSR. It proposes that, although sub-
    jects attempt TSR before stimulus onset (given the
    opportunity), they succeed on only a proportion of
    switch trials. If they succeed they are as ready for the

    changed task as on a task-repetition trial. If they ‘fail
    to engage’, the whole TSR process must be performed
    after stimulus onset. This idea of TSR as a probabil-
    istic all-or-none state change is supported by the fit of
    a discrete-state mixture model to the distribution of
    reaction times (RTs) on prepared switch trials [15,16].
    But why should TSR be all-or-none? One rationale is
    that TSR includes an attempt to retrieve either the
    goal or the task rules from memory; retrieval attempts
    either succeed or fail [17,18].

    Fig. 1. Predictable and unpredictable task switching. In this experiment (Ref. [42], Exp. 2), the tasks were to classify the digit as either odd/even or high/low, with a left or
    right key-press. (a) For some subjects, the task was cued by the background colour (as illustrated) and for others by the background shape; the colour or shape changed at
    the beginning of every trial. The response–stimulus interval in different blocks was 50 ms, 650 ms and 1250 ms, during which subjects could prepare for the next stimulus.
    In some blocks, the task changed predictably every four trials (with a ‘clock hand’ rotating to help keep track of the sequence): the ‘switch cost’ was limited to the first trial
    of the changed task (b). In other blocks, the task varied randomly from trial to trial and recovery from a task switch was more gradual. In both cases, the switch cost was
    reduced by ,50% by extending the time available for preparation to 650 ms (the ‘preparation effect’); a further increase had little effect (the ‘residual cost’). These data
    demonstrate that, at least in normal, young adults, even with complete foreknowledge about the task sequence, switch costs are large, and that recovery from a task switch
    is characteristically complete after one trial. When the task is unpredictable, recovery might be more gradual, but a few repetitions of a task results in asymptotic readiness
    for it. (Data redrawn with permission from Ref. [42].)

    TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences

    (a)

    Predictable task sequence

    Random task sequence

    Trial

    Cue (50, 650,
    or 1250 ms)

    Stimulus
    (until response)

    8

    6 8 1 3 8 4

    2 7 9 1 8 2

    (b)

    500

    600

    700

    800

    900

    1000 50

    650

    1250

    Predictable Random

    0.0

    2.0

    4.0

    6.0

    1 2 3 4

    Position in run

    0.0
    2.0
    4.0
    6.0
    1 2 3 4
    500
    600
    700
    800
    900

    1000

    E
    rr

    or
    s

    (%
    )

    M
    ea

    n
    co

    rr
    ec

    t R
    T

    (m
    s)

    Fig. 2. Preparation effect and residual cost. (a) In this experiment (Ref. [13], Exp. 3), the stimulus is a character pair that contains a digit and/or a letter. The tasks were to clas-
    sify the digit as odd/even, or the letter as consonant/vowel. The task changed predictably every two trials and was also cued consistently by location on the screen (rotated
    between subjects). (b) The time available for preparation (response–stimulus interval) varied between blocks. Increasing it to ,600 ms reduced switch cost (the ‘prep-
    aration effect’), but compared with non-switch trials there was little benefit of any further increase, which illustrates the ‘residual cost’ of switching. (Data redrawn with per-
    mission from Ref. [13].)

    TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences
    600
    650
    700

    750

    800

    850

    900

    0 500 1000 1500
    Response–stimulus interval (ms)

    Switch trial
    Non-switch trial

    M
    ea
    n
    co
    rr
    ec
    t R
    T
    (m
    s)

    (a) (b)

    G7 #E

    4A L9

    Letter task
    (switch)

    Letter task
    (non-switch)

    Digit task
    (switch)

    Digit task
    (non-switch)

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003136

    http://tics.trends.com

    http://www.trends.com

    Transient task-set inertia
    Consider Stroop stimuli. It is well-known that incongru-
    ence between the colour in which the word is displayed and
    the colour it names interferes much more with naming the
    display colour than with naming the word, an asymmetry
    of interference that is attributable to word naming being
    the more practised, and hence ‘stronger’, task-set [19].
    Surprisingly, if subjects must switch between this pair of
    tasks, switching to the stronger task results in the larger
    switch cost [20–22]. In another striking example, bilingual
    subjects named digits more slowly in their second langu-
    age on non-switch trials, but on switch trials named more
    slowly in their first language [23]. This surprising
    asymmetry of switch costs eludes explanation in terms of
    the duration of TSR. How could it take longer to
    reconfigure for the more familiar task? Allport et al. [20]
    propose that one must apply extra inhibition to the
    stronger task-set to enable performance of the weaker.
    This inhibition then carries over to the next trial;
    overcoming the inhibition prolongs response selection.

    Subsequent work reveals some problems with this
    account. For example, the surprising asymmetry of switch
    costs can be reversed by manipulations that produce only a
    modest reduction in the asymmetry of Stroop-like inter-
    ference between the tasks [22,24]. However, this pattern
    can be accommodated by a model that combines transient
    persistence of task-set activation (or inhibition) with the
    assumption that executive processes apply the minimum
    endogenous-control input that enables the appropriate
    task, given the anticipated interference [22]. The detection
    of cross-task interference during a trial might also prompt
    the ramping-up of endogenous control input, which would
    lead to greater TSI on a switch trial following an
    incongruent stimulus [9].

    Other observations support the transient carry-over of
    task-set activation from trial to trial. Several researchers
    [25,26] report evidence that, with preparation held
    constant, a longer delay after the last performance of the
    previous task improves performance on the switch trial.
    This suggests dissipating activation of the competing task-
    set. Sohn and Anderson [18] fit data on the interaction
    between preparation interval and foreknowledge with a
    model that assumes exponential decay of task-set acti-
    vation following a trial, and an endogenous preparation
    process whose probability of success increases throughout
    the preparation interval. There is also evidence for
    persistence of inhibition applied to a task-set in order to
    disengage from it: so, for example, responses are slower on
    the last trial of the sequence Task A, Task B, Task A, than
    the sequence Task C, Task B, Task A [27,28].

    Associative retrieval
    Even when performing only one task (e.g. word naming),
    responses are slower if subjects have performed another
    task afforded by the same stimuli (e.g. colour naming) in
    the previous few minutes [20,21,29]. This long-term
    priming has been attributed to associative retrieval of
    task-sets that are associated with the current stimulus
    [29,30], and seems likely to be the source of the mixing
    cost. Allport and colleagues found this priming to be
    magnified on a switch trial or when performance was

    merely resumed after a brief pause, which suggests that
    associative interference may contribute also to switch
    costs [21,29]. Further experiments [30] demonstrated that
    this priming can be quite stimulus-specific. In these
    experiments, each stimulus was a line drawing of one
    object with the name of another superimposed (e.g. a lion
    with the word APPLE). In the first block, subjects named
    the object, ignoring the word. Later, they showed larger
    switch costs for naming the word in stimuli for which they
    had previously named the picture, even if only once and
    several minutes before.

    All of the above?
    Initial theorising tended to try to explain switch costs in
    terms of just one mechanism (e.g. [13,20]). Although
    single-factor models of task switching continue to be
    proposed [31] most authors now acknowledge a plurality of
    causes, while continuing to argue over the exact blend. For
    example, although long-term effects of task priming imply
    associative activation of competing task-sets by the
    stimulus, the contribution this makes to the transient
    switch cost observed with small sets of stimuli, all recently
    experienced in both tasks, is uncertain. Moreover, residual
    switch costs occur even with ‘univalent’ stimuli (i.e. those
    associated with only one task) for which there should be no
    associative competition [13,26], and switch costs some-
    times do not occur for bivalent stimuli where there should
    be massive associative competition, such as switching
    between prosaccades and antisaccades to peripheral
    targets [32]. Transient carry-over of task-set activation
    or inhibition is now well established as an important
    contributor to switch costs, especially the residual cost, but
    it remains unclear whether the effect is to slow task-
    specific processes (e.g. response selection) or to trigger
    extra control processes (ramping up of control input when
    response conflict is detected). A combination of both
    mechanisms is likely. Something of a consensus has
    developed around the idea that the preparation effect, at
    least, reflects a time-consuming, endogenous, task-set-
    reconfiguration process, which, if not carried out before the
    stimulus onset, must be done after it.

    Issues for further research
    Unfortunately, the foregoing consensual account of the
    preparation effect is not without problems. First, there are
    studies in which the opportunity for preparation with
    either full [33] or partial [34] foreknowledge of the
    upcoming task does not reduce the switch cost, even
    though it improves overall performance. Second, in task-
    switching experiments, to know whether TSR is necessary,
    a subject must discriminate and interpret an external cue
    (with unpredictable switching), retrieve the identity of the
    next task from memory (with predictable switching), or
    both (many predictable switching experiments provide
    external cues as well). The contribution of these processes
    to switch costs has been neglected. Koch [35] has reported
    that, with predictable switching, a preparation interval
    reduces the switch cost only when there is an external cue
    to help subjects remember which task is next. Logan and
    Bundesen [36] found that changing the cue when repeat-
    ing the task produced nearly as much of a preparation

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003 137

    http://tics.trends.com

    http://www.trends.com

    effect as changing both cue and task. Hence, processes of
    interpreting the cue and/or determining whether TSR is
    required might contribute much of the preparation effect.
    It is even possible that, in some cases, these processes are
    so demanding that they constitute a separate task, thus
    vitiating the distinction between ‘switch’ and ‘non-switch’
    trials.

    Another intriguing issue is the role of language.
    Introspection indicates that in both everyday life and
    task-switching experiments people to some extent verbal-
    ize what they intend to do next (‘er…colour’) and how (‘if
    red, this key’). Goschke [9] found that requiring subjects to
    say an irrelevant word during a 1.5 s preparation interval
    abolished the reduction in switch cost observed when the
    subject either named the task (‘colour’ and ‘letter’) or said
    nothing. He attributed this to interference with verbal
    self-instruction, extending to TSR the Vygotskian view
    [37] that self-instruction using language is fundamental to
    self-regulation. Others have found that irrelevant con-
    current articulation (e.g. saying ‘one–two–one–two…’) –
    which is known to interfere with phonological working
    memory – impairs performance disproportionately in task
    alternation compared to single task blocks [38,39]. It is
    also suggested that the association claimed between
    damage to the left prefrontal cortex and switching deficits
    (see below) reflects impaired verbal mediation caused by
    left hemisphere damage, rather than a more general
    control deficit [40]. However, subjects in these studies were
    relatively unpractised. Traditional theories of skill acqui-
    sition [41] assign language a relatively transitory role in
    task-set learning. A task-set, especially if acquired via the
    verbal instructions of another person, may be represented
    initially via verbal self-instruction, but after sufficient
    practice, control shifts from declarative (including verbal)
    representations to a learned, procedural representation.
    Standard examples are learning to shift gear or tie a knot.
    Hence, we might expect that any cost or benefit of verbal
    self-instruction in reconfiguring a task-set would vanish
    with practice.

    Experiments on task switching have thrown up
    numerous other puzzling observations. Why does an
    opportunity for preparation often reduce switch costs
    without reducing Stroop-like interference from the other
    task [13,25,42]? Why are switch costs larger when the
    response is the same as the previous trial [13]? We are
    unlikely to make sense of the increasingly complex set of
    variables that are known to influence switch costs without
    either computational simulation [43,44] or mathematical
    modelling [18,22,45,46] of their interactions. Progress in
    disentangling the complex causation of switch costs is
    necessary to interpret the effects of ageing [47–49] and
    brain damage [50,51] on, and individual differences [52] in,
    task-switching costs, and their association and dis-
    sociation with behavioural indices of other control func-
    tions. Even without a full understanding of their
    causation, the substantial magnitude of switch costs
    should also be an important consideration in the design
    of human–machine interfaces that require operators to
    monitor multiple information sources and switch between
    different activities under time pressure, such as in air-
    traffic control.

    Brain correlates of task switching
    At first glance, task switching lends itself well to the
    subtractive methodology of neuroimaging and electro-
    physiology. We can compare event-related activation in
    trials that differ only in whether they do or do not follow
    another of the same task. Numerous brain regions, usually
    in medial and lateral regions of the prefrontal cortex, but
    sometime in parietal lobes, cerebellum and other sub-
    cortical regions, are reported to be more active on switch
    than on non-switch trials. As one example, left dorso-
    lateral prefrontal cortex has been reported to be more
    active when subjects switch the attribute attended to
    [53,54], and this appears consistent with evidence that
    patients with left frontal damage have behavioural
    abnormalities in switching between attributes [50,51].

    Regrettably, as we have learned from behavioural
    studies, task switch and repeat trials are likely to differ
    in ways other than the occurrence of TSR. There may be
    extra interference at the levels of both task-set and
    stimulus–response mapping. The greater difficulty of
    switch trials is likely to elicit general arousal and extra
    error-monitoring. Moreover, even if region X contains an
    executive ‘module’ that reconfigures the behaviour of
    regions A, B and C, we would expect to see differential
    activation, not only of the controlling region X, but also of
    areas A, B and C, much as we see modulation of activation
    in striate and extrastriate cortex when visual attention is
    shifted endogenously [55]. Differential activation evoked
    by stimuli on switch and repeat trials does not differentiate
    between the ‘source’ and the ‘target’ of the control.

    One approach is to try to isolate the brain activity that is
    associated with preparing for a task switch. By stretching
    out the preparation interval to 5 s [11], 8 s [12] and 12.5 s
    [54], one can try to separate modulations of the blood-
    oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal that are linked to
    preparatory activity from changes associated with process-
    ing of the stimulus on switch trials. Some have reported
    that preparation for a switch evokes extra activation in
    regions that are different from those that undergo extra
    activation to a switch-trial stimulus [11,54] whereas
    others have not [12]. However, long preparation intervals
    might either require extra processing to maintain prepar-
    ation, or encourage subjects to postpone preparation. To
    deal with this, Brass and von Cramon [56] compared
    activation in trials with a task cue followed by a stimulus
    1.2 s later, trials in which the stimulus was omitted, trials
    in which the cue was delayed until the stimulus onset, and
    null trials. Cue-only trials caused activation in the left
    inferior frontal junction and the pre-SMA region that
    correlated with the behavioural cueing benefit in cue-
    stimulus trials. When the cue was delayed, this activation
    was also delayed. Hence this activity seems to be cue-
    related, but it is unclear (as in behavioural studies)
    whether it is associated with interpreting the cue or the
    consequent TSR.

    In a study focusing on the medial frontal cortex,
    Rushworth et al. [57] interrupted a series of stimuli
    every 9–11 trials with a ‘stay/shift’ cue. When the cue
    indicated whether to maintain or reverse the left/right
    response rule in the following trials, a larger BOLD signal
    was evoked in the pre-SMA region by ‘shift’ than by ‘stay’

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003138

    http://tics.trends.com

    http://www.trends.com

    cues. When the cue specified whether to maintain or
    switch the stimulus dimension (colour versus shape) used
    to direct attention for a perceptual detection task, a
    more posterior ‘hot-spot’ was seen. To determine
    whether these activations were functionally essential,
    brief trains of TMS pulses were applied to these
    regions. TMS following a shift, but not a stay, cue
    substantially prolonged RT to the upcoming stimulus,
    but only for the response-rule reversal. Hence activity
    in the pre-SMA region is, apparently, needed to reverse
    a stimulus–response assignment. We do not know
    whether this activity reflects the source or the target of
    an ‘act of control’, or both.

    Acknowledgements
    Thanks to Hal Pashler, Nachshon Meiran, Ulrich Mayr and an anonymous
    reviewer for their comments on an earlier draft of this article.

    References
    1 Norman, D.A. and Shallice, T. (1986) Attention to action: willed and

    automatic control of behaviour. In Consciousness and Self-Regulation
    (Vol. 4) (Davidson, R.J. et al., eds), pp. 1–18, Plenum Press

    2 Monsell, S. (1996) Control of mental processes. In Unsolved Mysteries
    of the Mind: Tutorial Essays in Cognition (Bruce, V., ed.), pp. 93–148,
    Erlbaum

    3 Duncan, J. (1990) Goal weighting and the choice of behaviour in a
    complex world. Ergonomics 33, 1265–1279

    4 Cooper, R. and Shallice, T. (2000) Contention scheduling and the
    control of routine activities. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 17, 297–338

    5 Logan, G.D. (1985) Executive control of thought and action. Acta
    Psychol. 60, 193–210

    6 Hommel, B. (2000) The prepared reflex: automaticity and control in
    stimulus–response translation. In Control of Cognitive Processes:
    Attention and Performance XVIII (Monsell, S. and Driver, J., eds)
    pp. 247–273, MIT Press

    7 MacLeod, C.M. (1991) Half a century of research on the Stroop effect:
    an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203

    8 Lhermitte, F. (1983) ‘Utilization behaviour’ and its relation to lesions of
    the frontal lobes. Brain 106, 237–255

    9 Goschke, T. (2000) Intentional reconfiguration and involuntary
    persistence in task set switching. In Control of Cognitive Processes:
    Attention and Performance XVIII (Monsell, S. and Driver, J., eds)
    pp. 331–355, MIT Press

    10 Monsell, S. and Driver, J. (2000) Control of Cognitive Processes:
    Attention and Performance XVIII, MIT Press

    11 Sohn, M-H. et al. (2000) The role of prefrontal cortex and posterior
    parietal cortex in task switching. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 97,
    13448–13453

    12 Kimberg, D.Y. et al. (2000) Modulation of task-related neural
    activity in task-switching: an fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 10,
    189–196

    13 Rogers, R.D. and Monsell, S. (1995) The costs of a predictable
    switch between simple cognitive tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 124,
    207–231

    14 Rubinstein, J.S. et al. (2001) Executive control of cognitive
    processes in task switching. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept.
    Perform. 27, 763–797

    15 De Jong, R. (2000) An intention-activation account of residual switch
    costs. In Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance
    XVIII (Monsell, S. and Driver, J., eds) pp. 357–376, MIT Press

    16 Nieuwenhuis, S. and Monsell, S. (2002) Residual costs in task
    switching: testing the ‘failure to engage’ hypothesis. Psychonomic
    Bull. Rev. 9, 86–92

    17 Mayr, U. and Kliegl, R. (2000) Task-set switching and long-term
    memory retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 1124–1140

    18 Sohn, M-H. and Anderson, J.R. (2001) Task preparation and task
    repetition: two-component model of task switching. J. Exp. Psychol.
    Gen. 130, 764–778

    19 Cohen, J.D. et al. (1990) On the control of automatic processes: a

    parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychol.
    Rev. 97, 332–361

    20 Allport, D.A. et al. (1994) Shifting intentional set: exploring the
    dynamic control of tasks. Attention and Performance XV: Conscious
    and Nonconscious Information Processing (Umiltà, C., Moscovitch, M.,
    et al. eds), pp. 421–452, MIT Press

    21 Allport, A. and Wylie, G. (1999) Task-switching: Positive and negative
    priming of task-set. In Attention, Space and Action: Studies in
    Cognitive Neuroscience (Humphreys, G.W. et al., eds), pp. 273–296,
    Oxford University Press

    22 Yeung, N. and Monsell, S. Switching between tasks of unequal
    familiarity: the role of stimulus-attribute and response-set selection.
    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. (in press)

    23 Meuter, R.F.I. and Allport, A. (1999) Bilingual language-switching in
    naming: asymmetrical costs of language selection. J. Mem. Lang. 40,
    25–40

    24 Monsell, S. et al. (2000) Reconfiguration of task-set: is it easier to
    switch to the weaker task? Psychol. Res. 63, 250–264

    25 Meiran, N. et al. (2000) Component processes in task switching. Cogn.
    Psychol. 41, 211–253

    26 Ruthruff, E. et al. (2001) Switching between simple cognitive tasks: the
    interaction of top-down and bottom-up factors. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum.
    Percept. Perform. 27, 1404–1419

    27 Mayr, U. and Keele, S.W. (2000) Changing internal constraints on
    action: the role of backward inhibition. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 129, 4–26

    28 Mayr, U. (2002) Inhibition of action rules. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. 9,
    93–99

    29 Allport, A. and Wylie, G. (2000) Task-switching, stimulus-response
    bindings and negative priming. In Control of Cognitive Processes:
    Attention and Performance XVIII (Monsell, S. and Driver, J., eds)
    pp. 35–70, MIT Press

    30 Waszak, F. et al. Task switching and long-term priming: role of episodic
    bindings in task shift costs. Cogn. Psychol. (in press)

    31 Altmann, E.M. and Gray, W.D. (2002) Forgetting to remember: the
    functional relationship of decay and interference. Psychol. Sci. 13,
    27–33

    32 Hunt, A.R. and Klein, R.M. (2002) Eliminating the cost of task set
    reconfiguration. Mem. Cogn. 30, 529–539

    33 Sohn, M-H. and Carlson, R.A. (1998) Effects of repetition and
    foreknowledge in task-set recongfiguration. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn.
    Mem. Cogn. 26, 1445–1460

    34 Dreisbach, G. et al. (2002) Preparatory processes in the task switching
    paradigm: evidence from the use of probability cues. J. Exp. Psychol.
    Learn. Mem. Cogn. 28, 468–483

    35 Koch, I. The role of external cues for endogenous advance reconfigura-
    tion in task switching. Psychonomic Bull. Rev. (in press)

    36 Logan, G. and Bundesen, C. Clever homunculus: is there an
    endogenous act of control in the explicit task-cuing procedure.
    J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. (in press)

    37 Vygotski, L.S. (1962) Thought and Language, MIT Press
    38 Baddeley, A. et al. (2001) Working memory and the control of

    action: evidence from task switching. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130,
    641–657

    39 Emerson, M.J. and Miyake, A. The role of inner speech in task
    switching. J. Mem. Lang. (in press)

    40 Mecklinger, A. et al. (1999) Executive control functions in task
    switching: evidence from brain injured patients. J. Clin. Exp.
    Neuropsychol. 21, 606–619

    41 Fitts, P.M. and Posner, M.I. (1967) Human Performance, Brooks-Cole
    42 Monsell, S. et al. Task-set reconfiguration after a predictable or

    unpredictable task switch: is one trial enough? Mem. Cogn. (in press)
    43 Gilbert, S.J. and Shallice, T. (2002) Task-switching: a PDP model.

    Cogn. Psychol. 44, 297–337
    44 Kieras, D.E. et al. (2000) Modern computational perspectives on

    executive mental processes and cognitive control: where to from
    here? Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention and Performance
    XVIII (Monsell, S., Driver, J.S., et al. eds), pp. 681–712, MIT
    Press

    45 Logan, G.D. and Gordon, R.D. (2001) Executive control of visual
    attention in dual-task situations. Psychol. Rev. 108, 393–434

    46 Meiran, N. (2000) Modeling cognitive control in task-switching.
    Psychol. Res. 63, 234–249

    47 Kramer, A.F. et al. (1999) Task coordination and aging: explorations of

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003 139

    http://tics.trends.com

    http://www.trends.com

    executive control processes in the task-switching paradigm. Acta
    Psychol. 101, 339–378

    48 Kray, J. and Lindenberger, U. (2000) Adult age differences in task
    switching. Psychol. Aging 15, 126–147

    49 Mayr, U. (2001) Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role
    of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. Psychol.
    Aging 16, 96–109

    50 Rogers, R.D. et al. (1998) Dissociating executive mechanisms of task
    control following frontal lobe damage and Parkinson’s disease. Brain
    121, 815–842

    51 Keele, S.W. and Rafal, R. (2000) Deficits of task-set in patients with left
    prefrontal cortex lesions. In Control of Cognitive Processes: Attention
    and Performance XVIII (Monsell, S. and Driver, J.S., eds) pp. 627–651,
    MIT Press

    52 Miyake, A. et al. (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions
    and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable
    analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100

    53 Meyer, D.E. et al. (1998) The role of dorsolateral prefronatl cortex for
    executive cognitive processes in task-switching. J. Cogn. Neurosci.
    (Suppl. S), 106

    54 MacDonald, A.W. et al. (2000) Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral
    prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science
    288, 1835–1838

    55 Hopfinger, J.B. et al. (2000) Electrophysiological and neuroimaging

    studies of voluntary and reflexive attention. Control of Cognitive
    Processes: Attention and Performance XVIII (Monsell, S., Driver, J.,
    et al. eds), pp. 125–153, MIT Press

    56 Brass, M. and von Cramon, D.Y. (2002) The role of the frontal cortex in
    task preparation. Cereb. Cortex 12, 908–914

    57 Rushworth, M.F.S. et al. (2002) Role of the human medial frontal
    cortex in task switching: a combined fMRI and TMS study.
    J. Neurophysiol. 87, 2577–2592

    58 Woodworth, R.S. and Schlosberg, H. (1954) Experimental Psychology,
    Holt, Rhinehart & Winston

    59 Jersild, A.T. (1927) Mental set and shift. Archives Psychol., 89
    60 Biederman, I. (1972) Human performance in contingent information

    processing tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. 93, 219–238
    61 Shaffer, L.H. (1965) Choice reaction with variable S–R mapping.

    J. Exp. Psychol. 70, 284–288
    62 Biederman, I. (1973) Mental set and mental arithmetic. Mem. Cogn. 1,

    383–386
    63 Sudevan, P. and Taylor, D.A. (1987) The cuing and priming of cognitive

    operations. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 13, 89–103
    64 Meiran, N. (1996) Reconfiguration of processing mode prior to

    task performance. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 22,
    1423–1442

    65 Gopher, D. et al. (2000) Switching tasks and attention policies. J. Exp.
    Psychol. Gen. 129, 308–339

    Research Focus

    Within the, Update section of TICS, Research Focus articles highlight important and interesting
    recent research developments in cognitive science. The authors of the original research papers

    discussed may be invited to write a Response to the Research Focus article

    If you know of any research just published that you think should be discussed, please contact
    the Editor (tics@current-trends.com) with your suggested article and details of why you think

    the article deserves to be highlighted.

    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.7 No.3 March 2003140

    http://tics.trends.com

    http://www.trends.com

      Task switching
      Task switching: basic phenomena
      Switch cost (task-repetition benefit)
      Preparation effect
      Residual cost
      Mixing cost
      Sources of the switch cost
      Time taken by control operations
      Transient task-set inertia
      Associative retrieval
      All of the above?
      Issues for further research
      Brain correlates of task switching
      Acknowledgements
      References

    See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232496441

    Costs of a Predictable Switch Between Simple Cognitive Tasks

    Article  in  Journal of Experimental Psychology General · June 1995

    DOI: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

    CITATIONS

    2,633
    READS

    12,221

    2 authors:

    Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

    Inclusion for Dangerous and Severe Personality Disorder: Evaluation, Assessment and treatment (IDEA) project View project

    Spatial attention in task switching View project

    Robert Rogers

    School of Psychology

    172 PUBLICATIONS   17,654 CITATIONS   

    SEE PROFILE

    Stephen Monsell

    University of Exeter

    59 PUBLICATIONS   10,576 CITATIONS   

    SEE PROFILE

    All content following this page was uploaded by Robert Rogers on 09 September 2015.

    The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232496441_Costs_of_a_Predictable_Switch_Between_Simple_Cognitive_Tasks?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_2&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232496441_Costs_of_a_Predictable_Switch_Between_Simple_Cognitive_Tasks?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_3&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/project/Inclusion-for-Dangerous-and-Severe-Personality-Disorder-Evaluation-Assessment-and-treatment-IDEA-project?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/project/Spatial-attention-in-task-switching?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_9&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_1&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert-Rogers-12?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert-Rogers-12?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert-Rogers-12?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Stephen-Monsell?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_4&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Stephen-Monsell?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_5&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/institution/University_of_Exeter?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_6&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Stephen-Monsell?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_7&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Robert-Rogers-12?enrichId=rgreq-c999f17d4cb3d6d4d4f091e4adea0b4e-XXX&enrichSource=Y292ZXJQYWdlOzIzMjQ5NjQ0MTtBUzoyNzE3MDkzMjAyNDkzNDVAMTQ0MTc5MTk1ODQzOA%3D%3D&el=1_x_10&_esc=publicationCoverPdf

    View publication statsView publication stats

    https://www.researchgate.net/publication/232496441

    Order your essay today and save 25% with the discount code: STUDYSAVE

    Order a unique copy of this paper

    600 words
    We'll send you the first draft for approval by September 11, 2018 at 10:52 AM
    Total price:
    $26
    Top Academic Writers Ready to Help
    with Your Research Proposal

    Order your essay today and save 25% with the discount code GREEN